CO 00 64225 >m OSMANIA UNIVERSITY LIBRARY Call No. i?3'^r/ /f ^4 ^ Accession No.% ' Author nut This book should be.returrtedjt'iiu^-^fof^ ?he date hist marked below- CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE BY ELLSWORTH HUNTINGTON Late Research Associate in Geography in Yale University NEW HAVEN YALE UNIVERSITY PRESS LONDON GEOFFREY CUMBERLEGE OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Copyright, 1015, 1922, 1924-, by Yale University Press. Printed In the United Stales of America. Published, 1915. Second Printing, 19 16. Third Printing, 1919. Second and Enlarged Edition, 1922. Third Edition with Many New Chapters, \ 924. Third Edition, Second Printing, 193*3. Third Edition, Third Printing, 1935. Third Edition, Fourth Printing, September, 1939. Third Edition, Fifth Printing, April, 1945. Third Edition, Sixth Printing, April, 1948. All rights reserved. This book may not be reproduced, in whole or in part, in any form (except by reviewers for the public press), without written permission from the publishers. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Illustrations vii Preface to First Edition xi Preface to Third Edition xv Author's Bibliography xvii Chapter I. Introduction 1 Chapter II. Race or Place 30 Chapter III. The White Man in the Tropics . . 56 Chapter IV*. The Effect of the Seasons .... 76 Chapter V. The Effect of Humidity and Tempera- Jure 109 Chapter VI. Work and Weather 136 Chapter VII." Health and the Atmosphere . . . 153 Chapter VIII. Mortality, Moisture, and Variability 174 Chapter IX. Health and Weather 194 Chapter X: The Ideal Climate 220 Chapter XI. The Distribution of Civilization . . 240 Chapter XII. Vitality and Education in the United States 275 Chapter XIII. The Conditions of Civilization . . 291 Chapter XIV. The Shifting of Climatic Zones . . 315 Chapter XV. The Pulsatory Hypothesis and Its Critics 335 Chapter XVI. The Shifting Centers of Civilization . 347 Chapter XVII. Aboriginal America and Modern Aus- tralia 366 vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter XVIII. The Climatic Hypothesis of Civiliza- tion 387 Appendix 413 Index 433 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure 1. The Effect of the Seasons on Factory Operatives in Connecticut and at Pittsburgh 84 Figure 2a. Human Activity and the Seasons ... 92 Figure 2b. Human Activity and the Seasons ... 93 Figure 3. Seasonal Variations of Mental Compared with Physical Activity 105 Figure 4. Relative Humidity and Work in Connecticut 112 Figure 5. Average Weekly Temperature During the Summers of 1910-1913 in Connecticut . . 116 Figure 6. Effect of the Days of the Week on Piece- Workers 120 Figure 7. Variations in Daily Wages 122 Figure 8. Human Activity and Mean Temperature . 124 Figure 9. Growth of Wheat at Various Temperatures 129 Figure 10. Mean Temperature and Vital Processes in Plants, Animals, and Man 130 Figure 11. Human Activity and Change of Mean Tem- perature from Day to Day 140 Figure 12. The Stimulus of Storms 147 Figure 13. Seasonal Variations in Energy and Health . 154 Figure 14. Seasonal Variations in Conceptions and Deaths in Japan, 1901-1910 .... 159 Figure 15. Post-operative Death Rate at Boston in Relation to Humidity and Temperature . 180 Figure 16. Relation between Deaths from Pneumonia and Influenza and Interdiurnal Changes of Temperature 184 Figure 17. Correlation between Weather Elements and Daily Deaths in New York City, December to March 1882-1886 196 Vlll LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure 18a. Excess or Deficiency of Death Rates in Months of Extreme Weather in American Cities, 1900-1915 206 Figure 18b. Excess or Deficiency of Death Rates in Months of Extreme Weather in American Cities, 1900-1915 207 Figure 19. Correction for Effect of Other Climatic Factors 211 Figure 20. Excess or Deficiency of Deaths in Relation to Stormy Periods Lasting One Month, Two Months, ancLThree Months, 1900-1915 . 216 Figure 21. Net Effect of Weather in the United States 218 Figure 22. The Effect of Climate on Human Energy as Inferred from Work in Factories . . . 234 Figure 23. The Distribution of Civilization^n Europe 257 Figure 24. The Distribution of Civilization in Asia . 259 Figure 25. The Distribution of Civilization in Australia 260 Figure 26. The Distribution of Civilization in Africa 261 Figure 27. The Distribution of Civilization in South America 262 Figure 28. Civilization in North America, According to all Contributors 264 Figure 29. Civilization in North America, According to Twenty-five Americans 265 Figure 30. Civilization in North America, According to Seven British Contributors .... 268 Figure 31. Civilization in North America, According to Six Germanic Europeans 269 Figure 32. Civilization in North America, According to Six Latin Europeans and One Russian . 272 Figure 33. Civilization in North America, According to Five Asiatics 273 Figure 34. Climatic Energy in the United States on the Basis of Factory Work 276 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS IX Figure 35. Vitality in the United States, According to Life Insurance Statistics 276 Figure 36. Distribution of Climatic Health on Basis of Seasonal Variations in 33 Cities, 1900-1915 277 Figure 37. Civilization in the United States . . . 277 Figure 38. Distribution of Education in the United States 284 Figure 39. Percentage of Gainfully Employed Persons in the United States Engaged in Manu- facturing, 1919 284 Figure 40. Distribution of Climatic Energy in Europe 292 Figure 41. Distribution of Health in Europe . . . 293 Figure 42. Distribution of Civilization in Europe . . 293 Figure 43. The Distribution of Human Health and Energy on the Basis of Climate .... 295 Figure 44. The Distribution of Civilization . . . 295 Figure 45. Birthplaces of Persons of Unusual Ability in the United States 307 Figure 46. Changes of Climate in Western Asia and Growth of Trees in California . . . .318 Figure 47. Changes of Climate in California for 2,000 Years 321 Figure 48. The Shifting of the Storm Belt .... 358 PREFACE TO FIRST EDITION THIS volume is a product of the new science of geography. The old geography strove primarily to produce exact maps of the physical features of the earth's surface. The new goes farther. It adds to the physical maps an almost innumerable series showing the distribution of plants, animals, and man, and of every phase of the life of these organisms. It does this, not as an end in itself, but for the purpose of comparing the physi- cal and organic maps and thus determining how far vital phe- nomena depend upon geographic environment. Among the things to be mapped, human character as expressed in civilization is one of the most interesting and one whose distribution most needs explanation. The only way to explain it is to ascertain the effect of each of many cooperating factors. Such matters as race, religion, institutions, and the influence of men of genius must be considered on the one hand, and geographical location, topography, soil, climate, and similar physical conditions on the other. This book sets aside the other factors, except inci- dentally, and confines itself to climate. In that lie both its strength and weakness. When the volume was first planned, I contemplated a discussion of all the factors and an attempt to assign to each its proper weight. The first friend whom I con- sulted advised a directly opposite course, whereby the emphasis should be centered upon the new climatic facts which seem to afford ground for a revision of some of our old estimates of the relation between man and his environment. In writing the book I have growingly felt the wisdom of that advice, and have been impressed with the importance of concentration upon a single point, even at the expense of seeming to take a one-sided view. xii PREFACE TO FIRST EDITION If the reader feels that due weight is not given to one factor or another, he must remember that many unmentioned phases of the subject have been deliberately omitted to permit fuller emphasis upon the apparent connection between a stimulating climate and high civilization. The materials for this volume have been derived from a great variety of sources. Although personal observation and investi- gation are the basis of much that is here stated, still more has been derived from the world's general store of knowledge. Except in a few special cases I have not attempted to give references. To the general reader footnotes are not only useless, but often a distraction and nuisance. The careful student, on the other hand, cannot form a fair estimate of the hypothesis here pre- sented without reading previous publications in which I have set forth the reasons for many conclusions which are not fully discussed here for lack of space. These publications contain numerous references. Accordingly, the needs of the student will be met by giving a brief list of books and articles which have served as preliminary steps to the present volume. These publi- cations form a logical series with only such repetition as is necessary to make each a complete unit. It is scarcely necessary to add that the rapid growth of the subject during the past ten years has led to important modifications in some of the earlier conclusions. The facts set forth in this volume have by no means been derived wholly from observation and reading. Not far from a hundred people have given direct personal assistance. They are so numerous that it is impossible to mention them all by name. Therefore it seems best not to single out any for special thanks. Many of my colleagues among the Yale faculty and among the geographers of America have gone out of their way to offer sug- gestions, or friendly criticisms, or to bring to my attention publications and facts that might have escaped my notice. Others connected with such organizations as the Carnegie Insti- PREFACE TO FIRST EDITION xiii tution of Washington and the United States Weather Bureau have placed me under obligation by the kind way in which they have taken a personal rather than official interest in answering queries and providing data. Equally great courtesy has been shown by officers and other members of the teaching force at West Point and Annapolis, and by officials connected with various factories, including some whose figures it has not yet been possible to tabulate. Another large group comprises con- tributors to the map of civilization, many of whom devoted to this work time which they could ill afford. Lastly, I owe much to personal friends who fall in none of the groups already speci- fied. I suppose that the total time given to this book by all these scores of people makes their contribution larger than mine. My chief hope is that they may feel that their kindness has not been wasted. To each and all I can only express my deep sense of gratitude, and most of all to those whose advice from the be- ginning has done more than anything else to keep this book true to a single aim. E. H. Yale University, New Haven, Conn., July, 1915. PREFACE TO THIRD EDITION IN preparing the third edition of this book two circumstances have led to a radical revision. First, the World War has receded far enough into the background so that people's minds are once more able to concentrate upon the far-reaching prob- lems of science, instead of the temporary details associated with war. During the past three years this has led to a marked in- crease in the number of publications dealing with the problems of this book. Second, during the nine years since the first edition was issued many new facts have come to light which amplify and strengthen the general hypothesis of the effect of climate upon civilization. The present edition differs from the first in several important respects : 1. In the first edition inheritance, physical environment, and culture were recognized as the three main factors in determining the distribution of civilization. Physical environment was of course treated fully, since it is the main subject of the book. Enough was also said about human culture to show that I fully appreciate its importance, especially as an explanation of the difference between aboriginal America and the Old World. In- heritance, however, was dismissed briefly. In the present edi- tion it receives a good deal of emphasis, especially in the first chapter, which is almost wholly new. It would be emphasized much more strongly were it not that in The Character of Races I have devoted a whole book to the problem. That book and this are so closely allied that neither is complete without the other. 2. The relation of climate to health has been much discussed during the past nine years. Accordingly three new chapters, Vii xvi PREFACE TO THIRD EDITION to IX, have been added on this topic. They balance the three preceding chapters by discussing the manifold effects of cli- mate and weather upon man from the standpoint of disease and death, instead of from the standpoint of the day's work. 3. In the first edition of Civilization and Climate I assumed that historians and others would be more familiar with the evi- dence of climatic changes during historic times than is actually the case. Accordingly in Chapter XIV the hypothesis of pulsa- tory climatic changes is more fully elaborated than formerly, while Chapter XV, which is new, is devoted to criticisms of that hypothesis. 4. Chapters XVI and XVII are devoted to some of the main criticisms of the hypothesis that climate is one of the three main determinants of the distribution of civilization. An especially important new feature is a study of the white man in tropical Australia. 5. In addition to this a large number of minor points have been added. Hence, although certain sections such as Chapters III to VI remain practically unchanged, the book as a whole has a distinctly new aspect. In its present form the book does not insist as strongly as before upon the supreme importance of climate, but the arguments which lead to the conclusion that climate ranks with racial inheritance and cultural development as one of the three main factors in determining the distribution of civilization seem much stronger than previously. E. H. New Haven, September, 1924. AUTHOR'S BIBLIOGRAPHY THIS list contains the titles of publications of the author which deal with the problems of this book. For details as to many points here discussed, the reader is referred to these publications. (A) Books and longer articles on changes of climate and their effect on man: (1) EXPLORATIONS IN TURKESTAN. Publications 26 and 73 of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1905, pp. 157-317. (2) THE PULSE OF ASIA. Boston, Houghton Mifflin Company, 1907, 415 pp. (3) PALESTINE AND ITS TRANSFORMATION. Boston, Hough- ton Mifflin Company, 1911, 433 pp. (4) THE CLIMATIC FACTOR AS ILLUSTRATED IN ARID AMER- ICA. Publication 192 of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1914, 341 pp. (5) THE SOLAR HYPOTHESIS OF CLIMATIC CHANGES. Bulletin of the Geological Society of America, vol. 25, 1914, pp. 477- 590. (B) Shorter articles dealing with phases of the problem of climatic changes not treated under (A) : (6) THE BURIAL OF OLYMPIA. Geographical Journal, Lon- don, 1910, pp. 657-686. (7) THE OASIS OF KHARGA. Bulletin of the American Geo- graphical Society, New York, vol. 42, 1910, pp. 641-661. (8) TREE GROWTH AND CLIMATIC INTERPRETATIONS. In Post- glacial Climatic Changes, Publication No. 352, Carnegie Insti- tution of Washington, 1924. (In press.) (C) Books and articles dealing with climate and civilization: (9) PHYSICAL ENVIRONMENT AS A FACTOR IN THE PRESENT xviii AUTHOR'S BIBLIOGRAPHY CONDITION OF TURKEY. Journal of Race Development, Worces- ter, Mass., vol. 1, 1911, pp. 460-481. (10) GEOGRAPHICAL ENVIRONMENT AND JAPANESE CHAR- ACTER. Journal of Race Development, Worcester, Mass., vol. 2, 1912, pp. 256-281. (Reprinted in a volume entitled JAPAN AND JAPANESE AMERICAN RELATIONS, edited by G. H. Blakeslee, New York, 1912, pp. 42-67.) (11) CHANGES OF CLIMATE AND HISTORY. American Histori- cal Review, vol. 18, 1913, pp. 213-232. (12) THE ADAPTABILITY OF THE WHITE MAN TO TROPICAL AMERICA. In Latin America. Clark University Addresses, 1913, edited by G. H. Blakeslee, pp. 360-386. (13) THE GEOGRAPHER AND HISTORY. The Geographical Journal, London, January, 1914. (14) A NEGLECTED FACTOR IN RACE DEVELOPMENT. The Journal of Race Development, Worcester, Mass., vol. 6, 1915, pp. 167-184. (15) CLIMATIC VARIATIONS AND ECONOMIC CYCLES. The Geographical Review, vol. 1, March, 1916, pp. 192-201. (16) MAYA CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATIC CHANGES. Proceed- ings of the 19th International Congress of Americanists, Wash- ington, 1917, pp. 150-164. (17) CLIMATIC CHANGE AND AGRICULTURAL EXHAUSTION AS ELEMENTS IN THE FALL OF ROME. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, vol. 31, February, 1917, pp. 173-208. (18) THE RED MAN'S CONTINENT. New Haven, 1919. (19) PRINCIPLES OF HUMAN GEOGRAPHY. New York, 1920. (20) BUSINESS GEOGRAPHY. New York, 1922. (D) Books and articles published since the first edition of CIVI- LIZATION AND CLIMATE and dealing largely with the relation between climate and health: (21) WORLD POWER AND EVOLUTION. New Haven, 1919. (22) THE INTERPRETATION OF THE DEATH RATE BY CLIMO- GRAPHS. Modern Medicine, vol. 1, May, 1919. AUTHOR'S BIBLIOGRAPHY xix (23) THE CONTROL, OF PNEUMONIA AND INFLUENZA BY THE WEATHER. Ecology, vol. 1, January, 1920. (24) THE PURPOSE AND METHODS OF AIR CONTROL IN HOS- PITALS. The Modern Hospital, vol. 14, April, 1920. (25) METHODS OF AIR CONTROL AND THEIR RESULTS. The Modern Hospital, vol. 14, May, 1920. (26) THE RELATION OF HEALTH TO RACIAL CAPACITY: THE EXAMPLE OF MEXICO. The Geographical Review, vol. 11, April, 1921. (27) AIR CONTROL AS A MEANS OF REDUCING THE POST- OPERATIVE DEATH RATE. American Journal of Surgery, vol. 35, July and October, 1921. (28) INFLUENZA AND THE WEATHER IN THE UNITED STATES IN 1918. The Scientific Monthly, vol. 17, November, 1923. (29) CAUSES OF GEOGRAPHICAL VARIATIONS IN THE INFLU- ENZA EPIDEMIC OF 1918 IN THE CITIES OF THE UNITED STATES. Bulletin of the National Research Council, vol. 6, July, 1923. (E) Books and articles dealing with natural selection and racial character: (30) GEOGRAPHY AND NATURAL SELECTION. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 1924. (31) THE CHARACTER OF RACES. New York, 1924. (F) Books dealing zvith the nature and causes of climatic changes : (32) CLIMATIC CHANGES (with S. S Visher). New Haven, 1922. (33) EARTH AND SUN: AN HYPOTHESIS OF SUNSPOTS AND WEATHER. New Haven, 1923. CHAPTER 1 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE INTRODUCTION THE races of the earth are like trees. Each according to its kind brings forth the fruit known as civilization. As russet apples and pippins may grow from the same trunk, and as peaches may even be grafted on a plum tree, so the culture of allied races may be transferred from one to another. Yet no one expects pears on cherry branches, and it is useless to look for Slavic civilization among the Chinese. Each may borrow from its neighbors, but will put its own stamp upon what it obtains. The nature of a people's culture, like the flavor of a fruit, de- pends primarily upon racial inheritance, which can be changed only by the slow processes of biological variation and selection. Yet inheritance is only one of the factors in the development of civilization. Religion, education, government, and all of man's varied occupations, customs, and institutions his inherited culture as the anthropologists say form a second great group of social influences whose power seems almost immeasurable. They do for man what cultivation does for an orchard. One tree may bear a few wormy, knotty little apples scarcely fit for the pigs, while another of the same variety is loaded with great red- cheeked fruit of the most toothsome description. The reason for the difference is obvious. One tree grows in a grassy tangle of bushes with no room to develop, little chance to get sunlight, and scant opportunity to obtain nourishment because of the abundance of other plants and the poverty and thinness of the unfertilized soil. The other stands in the midst of a carefully 2 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE tilled garden where it has plenty of room to expand and enjoy the sun, and where its roots can spread widely in a deep, mellow, well-fertilized soil. Moreover, one tree is burdened with dead wood and suckers, and infested with insects and other parasites ; while the other is carefully pruned, scraped, and sprayed. In spite of the most careful and intelligent cultivation, a tree of the finest variety may fail to produce good fruit. Too much rain or too little ; prolonged heat or constant cloudiness ; frost when the blossoms are opening, or violent wind and hail may all be disastrous. The choicest tree without water is worth less than the poorest where the temperature and rainfall are propitious. Its health is ruined, and it can bear no fruit. Here, as in the preceding case, the great need of the tree is health in the fullest and broadest sense. A good climate, good cultivation, and good nourishment are merely means of giving the tree perfect health and thus allowing the fullest development of its inheritance. Thus the two great factors which really determine the quality of the fruit are inheritance and health. The other factors, namely, food, climate, parasites, and cultivation, are important chiefly as means whereby health, or, perchance, inheritance, is modified. Does the fruit known as civilization depend upon these same conditions? It seems to me that it does. Few would question that a race with a superb mental and physical inheritance and en- dowed with perfect health is capable of adding indefinitely to the cultural inheritance received from its ancestors, and thus may attain the highest civilization. But if that same cultural inheritance were given to a sickly race with a weak inheritance of both mind and body, there would almost surely be degenera- tion. Aside from biological inheritance, the main factors in determining health are climate, food, parasitic diseases, and a people's stage of culture, which corresponds to the cultivation of the tree. Moreover, these same four factors, through their potency in selecting some types for preservation and others for CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 3 destruction, and perhaps through their power to cause muta- tions, are among the main agencies in determining inheritance. Climate stands first, not because it is the most important, but merely because it is the most fundamental. It is fundamental by reason of its vital influence upon the quantity and quality not only of man's food but of most of his other resources ; it plays a large part in determining the distribution and virulence of the parasites which cause the majority of diseases ; and through its effect upon human occupations, modes of life, and habits, it is one of the main determinants of culture. On the other hand, neither food, disease, nor culture has any appreciable effect upon climate, although they may modify its influence. Moreover, climate has a direct effect upon health in addition to its indirect effect through food, disease, and mode of life. Hence although climate may be no more important than other factors in deter- mining the relative degree of progress in different parts of the world, it is more fundamental in the sense that it is a cause rather than a result of the other factors. In studying climate it is essential to draw a sharp distinction between three types of influences. In the first place climate has a direct effect upon man's health and activity. Second, it has a strong indirect but immediate effect through food and other resources, through parasites, and through mode of life. Third, by its combined direct and indirect effects in the past it has been a strong factor some would say, the strongest in caus- ing migration, racial mixture, and natural selection ; and it may have had something to do with producing the variations which the biologists call mutations. Thus it has had a powerful effect upon inheritance. From the days of Aristotle to those of Montesquieu and Buckle, there have been good thinkers who have believed that the direct effect of climate is the most important factor in de- termining the differences between the degree of progress in vari- ous parts of th? earth. Others have held that wherever food is 4 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE available for a moderately dense population and man can avoid diseases like tropical malaria, human culture can rise to the highest levels. The location of the world's great nations seems to them largely a matter of accident. The majority of people reject both of these extreme views. Few doubt that climate has an important relation to civilization, but the majority consider it less important than racial inherit- ance, proper food, or good institutions in the form of church, state, and home. We realize that a dense and progressive popula- tion does not live in the far North or in deserts simply because l 1 he difficulty of getting a living grinds men down and keeps them isolated. We know that the denizens of the torrid zone are slow and backward, and we almost universally agree that this is connected with the damp, steady heat. We continually give concrete expression to our faith in climate. Not only do we talk about the weather more than about any other one topic, but we visit the seashore or the mountains for a change of air. We go South in winter, and to cool places in summer. We are depressed by a series of cloudy days, and feel exuberant on a clear, brac- ing morning after a storm. Yet, in spite of this universal recog- lition of the importance of climate, we rarely assign to it a 'oremost place as a condition of civilization. We point out that cpreat nations have developed in such widely diverse climates as v he hot plains of Mesopotamia and Yucatan and the cool hill country of Norway and Switzerland. Moreover, although Illi- nois and southern Mongolia lie in the same latitude and have the same mean temperature, they differ enormously in civiliza- tion. To put the matter in another way, we recognize two great sets of facts which are apparently contradictory. We are con- scious of being stimulated or depressed by climatic conditions, *nd we know that as one goes northward or southward, the dis- tribution of civilization is generally in harmony with what we should expect on the basis of our own climatic experiences. Nevertheless, even in our own day, regions which lie in the same CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 5 latitude and apparently have equally stimulating climates differ greatly in their degree of civilization. When we compare the past with the present, we find the same contradiction still more distinctly marked. Hence our confusion. From personal experi- ence we know that the direct effects of climate are of tremendous importance. Yet many facts seem to indicate that this impor- tance is less than our observation would lead us to anticipate. The reason for this doubtful attitude can easily be discovered. The things that we call facts are often not well established. Although we believe in the influence of climate, we know little of the particular climatic elements which are most stimulating or depressing. How much do we know of the relative importance of barometric pressure, wind, temperature, or humidity ? What about the comparative effects of the climates of England and southeastern Russia? In addition to this, we are far from know- ing what type of climate prevailed in Egypt, Greece, or Mespo- tamia when they rose to eminence. Many good authorities have asserted that the climate of those regions was the same two or three thousand years ago as now. This view is rapidly losing ground, but those who believe in a change are not certain of its nature. They are not yet wholly agreed as to whether it has produced an important influence upon the particular climatic elements which are most stimulating to the human system. This book has been written because two recent lines of in- vestigation apparently combine to explain at least part of the contradictions which have hitherto proved so puzzling. In the first place a prolonged study of past and present climatic varia- tions led to the conclusion that the climate of the past was dif- ferent from that of the present. In early historic times, for example, some parts of the world appear to have been drier than now* and other parts moister. In any given place, however, the change from the past to the present has not consisted of ~* eadily progressive tendency in one direction but of variations. In the places that were formerly moister than now there appear 6 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE to have been alternate changes toward dryness and then toward moisture ; while in the places that are drier than in the past there have been corresponding variations of the opposite types. In a word, the climate of historic times seems to have undergone a pronounced series of pulsations which have varied in char- acter from one part of the earth to another. The second line of investigation which originally led to the writing of this book was a study of the climatic conditions under which people of European races are able to accomplish the most work and have the best health. This investigation led to the conclusion that the principle of climatic optima applies to man quite as fully as to plants and animals. According to this principle each living species has the best health and is most active under certain definite conditions of temperature, humidity, wind movement, storminess, variability, and sunlight, or, more exactly, under certain combinations of these conditions. Any departure from the optimum conditions leads to a decrease of activity and effi- ciency. During the last ten years the importance of racial inheritance and racial selection has been strongly emphasized. In the first edition of Civilization and Climate the importance of race is strongly emphasized, but I failed to see how important a part has been played by climatic changes in selecting certain types of people for destruction or preservation. Such selection is ap- parently one of the chief ways in which the character of races is altered. The climatic pulsations of the glacial, post-glacial, and historic periods appear to have exerted a profound influence upon the degree of habitability of different parts of the earth. Thus famine, distress, and disease have arisen, and the pressure of population has led to migration, racial mixture, and the pres- ervation of one type of people in one place and another some- where else. Natural selection under the stress of climate goes far toward explaining many of the cases where the distribution of civilization does not agree with what would be expected on the CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 7 basis of the direct effect of climate. So important is this that I have written a book on The Character of Races for the ex- press purpose of applying the principles of natural selection to the history of racial development. That book might well have been called Civilization and Race in order to emphasize the fact that it is a continuation of the present work. The change in my own realization of the part played by climatic changes is one of the chief reasons why the present edition of Civilization and Climate is in some respects almost a new book. A large part of the reasoning of this book stands or falls with the hypothesis of climatic pulsations in historic times. The steps which led to the hypothesis may be briefly sketched as follows : In 1903, under the inspiration of the broad vision of Raphael Pumpelly and the careful scientific methods of William Morris Davis, I began to study the climate of the past. Two years' work with the Pumpelly Expedition sent to Turkestan by the Carnegie Institution of Washington led to the conviction that Reclus, Kropotkin, and others are correct in believing that two or three thousand years ago the climate of Central Asia was moister than now, a view which I advocated in Explorations in Turkestan. Later, during the Barrett Expedition to Chinese Turkestan, it became evident that the scientists who hold that the ancient climate in those regions was as dry as that of today also have much strong evidence to support their view. It soon appeared, however, that this apparent contradiction is fully explained by the fact that throughout the dry regions of Central Asia and the eastern Mediterranean the evidences of moist and dry con- ditions, respectively, are grouped in distinct periods ; the begin- ning of the Christian era was moist, for example, and the seventh century dry. This led to what I have called the "pulsatory hypothesis," which furnished a name for The Pulse of Asia. According to this hypothesis, although the historic and pre- historic past in those particular regions was in general moister than the present, the change from moist to dry has taken place 8 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE irregularly in great waves. Even in early historic times certain centuries were apparently drier than today, while others not long ago were moist. In 1909, this view was confirmed during the Yale Expedition to Palestine, the results of which are set forth in Palestine and Its Transformation. Then a series of journeys in the drier parts of the United States and in Mexico and Central America, in cooperation with D. T. MacDougal of the Desert Botanical Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution, showed that the main features of previous conclusions apparently apply to the New World as well as the Old. The most important feature of this work in America was the measurement of the thickness of the annual rings of growth of some four hundred and fifty of the Big Trees of California the Sequoia Washingtoniana, which grows high in the Sierras. These measurements made it possible to form a fairly reliable climatic curve for 2000 years and an approximate curve for another thousand. The final data as to the Big Trees were pub- lished in The Climatic Factor, which appeared in 1915, at the same time as Civilization and Climate. The agreement of the California curve with the climatic curve of western Asia as pre- viously worked out, and the constantly growing evidence as to the reliability of tree growth as a measure of climate have done far more than anything else to cause the hypothesis of climatic mlsations to be widely accepted. Here is the way the matter is summed up by the British meteorologist, C. E. P. Brooks, in his book on The Evolution of Climate (1922) : "The question of climatic changes during the historic period has been the subject of much discussion, and several great meteorologists and geog- raphers have endeavoured to prove that at least since about 500 B.C. there has been no appreciable variation. It is admitted that there have been shiftings of the centers of population and civi- lization, first from Egypt and Mesopotamia to the Mediterra- nean regions, and later to northern and western Europe, but these have been attributed chiefly to political causes and es- CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 9 pecially to the rise of Islam and the rule of the 'accursed Turk. 1 * Recently, however, there has arisen a class of evidence which cannot be explained away on political grounds, and which ap- pears to have decided the battle in favour of the supporters of change; I refer to the evidence of the trees. The conclusions derived from the big trees of California have fallen admirably into line with archaeological work in Central America, in central Asia and other regions, and have shown that the larger varia- tions even of comparatively recent times have been very exten- sive, if not world-wide, in their development." Another important factor in perfecting the pulsatory hy- pothesis has been the study of the Maya ruins in Yucatan and Guatemala. They join with other evidence in suggesting that changes of climate are of different types in different parts of the world. Central America seems to have been relatively dry at times when the Big Trees of the Sierras suggest that Cali- fornia was moist. This is an important modification of some of the conclusions which I have seemed to imply in earlier books. At practically the same time when this newer conclusion was published, an almost identical idea was presented by J. W. Gregory of England, whose article 7s the Earth Drying Up? in the Geographical Journal for 1914 is the strongest criticism of my climatic theories that has ever appeared. It will be discussed more fully later. The fact that two investigators who seemed to be opposed should independently publish the same conclusion without knowing what the other was doing greatly strengthens the force of that conclusion. Having reached the conclusion that pulsatory climatic changes have taken place during historic times and have differed in type from region to region, the next step was to study the mechanism and cause of the supposed changes. In Palestine and the eastern Mediterranean the conditions of vegetation, espe- cially the palm and vine, as Gregory has well shown, make it almost certain that variations in storminess and rainfall, rather 10 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE than in temperature, have been the primary factor. The recorded observations upon the mild climatic pulsations of the past hun- dred years support this conclusion. Various lines of evidence also indicate that climatic pulsations probably consist of a shifting of the earth's climatic zones or at least of the areas of cyclonic storms alternately toward and away from the equator. The idea as to zones, although not as to storms, was announced by the German geologist, Penck, at essentially the same time that I announced it, but the two conclusions were wholly inde- pendent and were based on quite different data. In both cases it was specifically recognized that the same kinds of climatic shiftings have taken place both in prehistoric and historic times, although the earlier changes were of greater magnitude. In these shiftings the temperate zone of storms appears to have been shoved irregularly back and forth. When it was farther south than at present, the subtropical countries which now are subarid must have been relatively moist. At the same time the subtropical arid belt was apparently sliif ted toward the equator so that on the borders of the torrid zone certain lands which now are wet were then relatively dry. When the shifting of zones took place in the opposite direction the reverse changes of climate appar- ently took place. It was only after the preceding conclusions as to climatic pulsations had reached essentially their present form that I began the next phase of the investigation, namely, the study of the actual effect of present climates upon human health and activity. This is important because some critics have supposed that I have unduly emphasized the importance of climatic changes, or have even formed a theory in regard to them for the purpose of bolstering up a preconceived idea that differences of climate from place to place are a main cause of the present dis- tribution of human progress. On the contrary, up to this period my reasoning had been somewhat as follows : If climatic changes have occurred during historic times, they must have had some CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 11 economic effect because such changes alter the capacity of a region to support population. The economic changes in their turn must have led to political disturbances and migrations. Is there any evidence of such events at times when the climate suffered unusually great or rapid changes? The possibility of such a connection between climate and history has deeply interested a great number of students. Kro- potkin, for instance, has vividly portrayed the wav in which a gradual desiccation of Asia presumably drove into Europe the hordes of barbarians whose invasions were so important a fea- ture of the Dark Ages. If the change from the climate of the past to that of the present has been marked by pulsations rather than by a progressive change in only one direction and if there have been certain periods of rather rapid change and of great, though temporary, extremes, as seems highly probable, the cor- respondence between historic events and climatic vicissitudes may be closer than would otherwise seem credible. Indeed, as soon as I had framed a preliminary outline of the curve of climatic changes during historic times, it appeared as though many of the great nations of antiquity, had risen or fallen in harmony with favorable or unfavorable conditions of climate. During periods of drought, not only are the people of the drier regions forced to migrate, especially if they are nomads, but increasing aridity, even in more favored places such as Greece, must cause economic distress, and thus engender famine, misery, and general discontent and lawlessness. A recent journey to China, which gave an opportunity for a study of the famines and barbarian invasions that have afflicted that country for two thousand years, has added greatly to the already abundant evi- dence of the truth of this view. It has also emphasized the re- markably intimate connection between economic distress and political discontent, a connection which is obvious in advanced countries like the United States, Australia, and Europe as well as in backward regions like China, Persia, India, and Mexico. 12 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE While these economic and political effects of climatic changes were being studied I became more and more impressed by the fact that when each country rose to a high level of civilization, it appears in a general way to have enjoyed a climate which approached more closely than now to certain well-defined con- ditions. These conditions appeared to resemble, but by no means duplicate, those now prevailing in most of the regions where civilization is highest. In spite of marked variations, the general tendency during periods of high civilization has apparently been toward cool, but not extremely cold, winters, and toward sum- mers which though warm or even hot for several months are generally varied by storms or at least by winds which produce frequent changes of temperature. It became especially evident that a relatively high degree of storminess and a relatively long duration of the season of cyclonic storms have apparently been characteristic of the places where civilization has risen to high levels both in the past and at present. Hence such places experi- ence much variability, a condition which later work has led me to believe highly beneficial. Up to this point in my investigations, I saw no ground for appealing to anything except economic and political factors in explanation of the apparent connection between civilization and climate. Then a little book on Malaria: A Neglected Factor in the History of Greece and Rome, by W. H. S. Jones, convinced me that climatic changes have altered the conditions of health as well as the economic situation. Later studies indicate that in other countries such as Central America, Indo-China, Java, and Egypt, as well as Greece and Rome, changes in the amount and virulence of such diseases as malaria and yellow fever may have been potent factors in diminishing the vitality of a nation. In fact it now seems probable that through their effect on bacteria, on the water supply, on the breeding places of insects, on the quality of the food, and perhaps in other ways climatic changes CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 13 may exert quite as much effect as through the more direct eco- nomic channels. The study of diseases was the natural prelude to a closer inquiry into the fact that at times of favorable climate in coun- tries such as Egypt and Greece the people were apparently filled with a virile energy which they do not now possess. Many authorities attribute the loss of this to an inevitable decay which must overtake a nation as old age overtakes an individual. Others ascribe it to the lack of adaptability in various institutions, to increasing luxury, to contact with inferior civilizations, to a change in racial inheritance, or to various other factors, most of which are doubtless of importance. Previous to 1911 a few authorities such as 0. Fraas had connected the decline of energy in Egypt, for example, with climatic changes, but they gave so few reasons and the whole matter seemed so doubtful that I had little faith in their suggestions. At that time, Charles J. Kull- mer of Syracuse University sent me a manuscript calling atten- tion to the remarkable similarity between the distribution of cyclonic storms and of civilization. His article was never pub- lished, but was presented at a meeting of the Association of American Geographers. He advanced the idea that the baro- metric changes which are the primary cause of storms, or perhaps some electrical phenomena which accompany them, may produce a stimulus which has much to do with the advancement of civilization. Although he presented no definite proof, his sug- gestion seemed so important that I determined to carry out a plan which had long been in mind. This was to ascertain the exact effect of different types of climate by means of precise measurements. Dexter, in his book on Weather Influences, had made a beginning. Lehmann and Pedersen had made a small series of measurements whose highly suggestive results have been published under the title Das Wetter und imsere Arbeit. A few physicians and students of child psychology were also at work, and their results have been summed up in such publica- 14 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE tions as Hellpach's GeopsycJiische Erscheinungen and Berliner's Einfluss von Klima, Wetter und Jahreszeit auf das N erven- und Seelen-leben. Nevertheless, there existed no large series of meas- urements of the actual efficiency of ordinary people under dif- ferent conditions of climate. The ideal way to determine the effect of climate would be to take a given group of people and measure their activity daily for a long period, first in one climate, and then in another. This, however, would not be practicable because of the great expense, and still more because the results would be open to question. If people were thus moved from place to place, it would be almost impossible to be sure that all conditions except climate remained uniform. If the climate differed markedly in the two places, the houses, food, and clothing would also have to be different. Social conditions would change. New interests would stimulate some people and depress others. Hence no such experiment now seems practicable. The most available method is apparently to take a group of people who live in a variable climate, and test them at all seasons. The best test is a man's daily work, the thing to which he devotes most of his time and energy. Accordingly, I took the records of four groups of people ; namely, some five hundred factory opera- tives in three Connecticut cities, New Haven, New Britain, and Bridgeport; nearly nine thousand operatives at Pittsburgh; three or four thousand operatives in southern cities from Vir- ginia to Florida; and over seventeen hundred students at the United States Naval Academy at Annapolis, and the Military Academy at West Point. In most cases each person's record covered an entire year, or at least the academic year. All the records were compared with the weather, as explained in later chapters. The results were surprising. Changes in the barometer seemed to have little effect. Humidity apparently possesses con- siderable importance, but the most important element is clearly temperature. The people here considered were phvsicallv most CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 15 active when the average temperature ranged from 60 to 65 F., that is, when the noon temperature rose to 70 or more, while the night temperature fell to 55 or so. This is higher than many of us would expect. Mental activity, however, reached a maximum when the outside temperature averaged about 38 F., that is, when there were mild frosts at night. Another highly important climatic condition seemed to be the change of temperature from one day to the next. People did not work well when the tempera- ture remained constant. Great changes were also unfavorable. The ideal condition, or optimum, seemed to be mild winters with some frosts, mild summers with the temperature rarely above 75 F., and a constant succession of mild storms and moderate changes of weather from day to day. The facts just stated seem to be of great significance as will be fully explained in this book. They suggest that the weather exerts a rather large and easily measurable effect upon man's capacity for both physical and mental work. This conclusion naturally led to a query as to how the climates in different parts of the world vary in their effect on human efficiency. Accord- ingly, I constructed a map showing how human energy would be distributed throughout the world if all the earth's inhabitants were influenced as were the fifteen thousand people of the four American groups mentioned above. This map was found to agree to a remarkable extent with a map of the present distribution of civilization based on the opinions of about fifty geographers and other widely informed men in a dozen countries of America, Europe, and Asia. Moreover, it agreed with the conclusions previously drawn as to the relation of climatic changes to the civilization of the past. Take, for example, the decadent coun- tries as to whose past climate we have some definite idea. In practically every case the climate during their more flourishing periods appears to have approached the optimum, as determined in the United States, more nearly than at present. This does not mean that the climate of Egypt, North India, China, 16 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Greece, or the Maya regions in Guatemala was ever like that of either New York or California. It merely means that it ap- proached more closely than at present to one or the other of these American types. Hence at the time of its greatness each region apparently enjoyed more than its present advantages in eco- nomic conditions, in freedom from parasitic diseases, and in direct climatic stimulus. When my investigations had reached this point, the first edi- tion of Civilization and Climate was written. During the nine or ten years that have since elapsed not only has much new evidence come to light, but my own point of view has changed consider- ably. The changes are set forth in the series of books and articles listed in the preface to the present (third) edition. They are referred to at some length in later chapters, but may here be briefly summarized. The comments on Civilization and Climate by historians and others make it more and more evident that the crux of the hy- pothesis of this book lies in changes of climate. The question, however, is not whether the climate of ancient Egypt, for ex- ample, was like that of modern England. It certainly was not and never could be. The contrast of the two countries in latitude, topography, and relation to land and sea makes any such close resemblance impossible. The real question is this : When Egypt rose to its greatest heights did its climate approach appreciably nearer than now to the type which provides the optimum condi- tions of energy, health, and economic strength for a people in the early Egyptian stage of development? Bear in mind that when the ancient Egyptians first rose to a state approaching civilization, they had not yet learned to use iron tools. Even in later days they had nothing like our modern skill in using wood and coal for heating houses, in manufacturing cotton and woolen cloth on such a scale that even the poor can be warmly clothed, in building houses that are proof against wind, rain, and cold ; nor had they our skill in combating disease. Hence their stage CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 17 of development caused the optimum climate to be warmer for them than for us. We are able to guard ourselves against low temperature and exposure, and thus gain an important stimulus without suffering much harm. They could not withstand cold winters. Bearing in mind then, that the optimum climate varies accord- ing to a nation's stage of civilization and also that there is doubtless some difference in the optimum from race to race, our problem becomes to determine how far the climate of the past in any given region was like that which is best for the stage of human progress, and perhaps the race, with which we happen to be dealing. This point of view is slightly different from that of the first edition of this book. The change is due largely to GilFillan's article on The Coldward Course of Progress in the Political Science Quarterly, 1920. So far as changes of climate are concerned, the ten years since this book was written have seen considerable new evidence as to the reality and nature of historic as well as prehistoric pulsa- tions. As an entire new chapter will be devoted to this matter, and as the comment of Brooks on the convincing character of the evidence has already been quoted, it is enough to point out here an interesting fact as to the kind of people who have ac- cepted the conclusions of this book. The non-scientific public, which has doubtless been the widest audience of Civilization and Climate, has accepted the book on the reasonableness of its main hypothesis, and with an open mind as to future proof or dis- proof. Geologists, archaeologists, and those geographers who have had a geological training are the types of scientists who have found the hypothesis most convincing. This is because most of the methods of reasoning and lines of evidence employed in discussions of climatic changes are such as are commonly used in geology, archaeology, and geography. The evidence consists chiefly of ancient lakes and streams, old roads in deserts, dead vegetation and its rings of annual growth, abandoned fields and 18 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE irrigation systems, and especially ruins and other traces of man, which are really human fossils. Such evidence appeals to geolo- gists, archaeologists, and geologically trained geographers. Anthropologists, economists, and historians, on the other hand, have been slow to believe that climatic changes have had much influence upon human history. They accept, indeed, the geologists' conclusion that previous to recorded history great climatic changes drove man this way and that, destroyed ancient types of culture, and either wiped whole races out of existence, or profoundly modified them, physically, mentally, and socially. They apparently have no difficulty in accepting the geological evidence that among primitive men, as among plants and animals, climate has been one of the most powerful factors in determining the distribution and vigor of different types. But when it comes to the period of written history many historians, and some anthropologists and economists, no longer trust the geological methods of reasoning. Their opinions are more or less unconsciously molded by two widely accepted assumptions. The first assumption is that climatic uniformity is a normal condition. This idea seems wholly untenable in view of the con- stantly growing evidence of numerous and important glacial periods and other extreme types of climate at all stages in geo- logical history. The more we know of the geological record, the more clear it becomes that change, not uniformity, is the rule. Even in the long periods when the larger types of climatic changes have been absent, there is abundant evidence of minor fluctuations and pulsations. The second assumption is equally doubtful. It holds that written records and statistics are more reliable than the geological type of evidence. Of course written records and statistics are far more reliable than any other types of evidence if they are sufficiently full, if they can be trusted, and if they are prepared by people who are conscious of the purposes for which they are to be used. But the written and recorded evidence as to the climate of the past consists of mere CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 19 scraps of information set down in most cases accidentally and with no idea of their possible significance in the distant future. Such evidence has, of course, great value, and must be studied assiduously. Nevertheless, it is inevitably subordinate to the geological type of evidence, which may be either physiographic, botanical, or archaeological. It seems clear, then, that the ultimate decision as to whether climatic changes have taken place on a large scale during his- torical times does not rest with historians. It rests primarily with persons who are trained in climatological, and especially geological, methods. During the last ten years geographers as a whole, in spite of some exceptions, seem to have become per- suaded that the historical period has witnessed a series of cli- matic pulsations like those of the prehistoric or post-glacial period although on a smaller scale. According to their own written statement in answer to a questionnaire, over nine tenths of the geographers of America, if we may judge from the fifty who have expressed their opinions, hold this view, although they are not quite so fully agreed as to how great the effect of these pulsations upon man may have been. Of course it is possible that a few geographers failed to answer the questionnaire be- cause they did not wish to express an opinion contrary to mine. I think, however, that the number of such is very limited, for most of those who are known to hold views unlike my own ex- pressed them freely. Even when all due allowance is made for failures to answer, it seems clear that among the people who are best competent to weigh the evidence the great majority believe in pulsations of climate. If these geographers with a geological training are right, there seems to be no escape from the conclusion that during certain periods of ancient history the climate of places like Egypt, Mesopotamia, and North India approached the optimum more closely than at present. Since the lower stage of culture in those early days presumably caused the optimum temperature for human progress to be higher than 20 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE is now the case among the most advanced races, the climatic conditions were even more favorable than I realized when this book was first written. If this conclusion is well grounded, it becomes a basic fact which the historian must take into account just as every careful student of early man now continually takes account of the fact that primitive man was greatly influenced by the vicissitudes of the successive glacial epochs, and just as every economist recognizes that modern man, on a smaller scale, is profoundly influenced by good or bad crops. History can never be written correctly until its physical basis is thoroughly understood, and until it is recognized that economic conditions, and human health and energy vary from age to age almost as much as do the conditions of politics, religion, and personality. Another line where there has been much progress in the last ten years is the determination of the nature and importance of the climatic optimum for man's physical development and health as opposed to his economic development. After Civilization and Climate had been completed, I undertook a study of the rela- tion of deaths to climate. The results appeared in World Power and Evolution. Some eight and a half million deaths in about sixty cities in the United States and thirty in France and Italy were analyzed according to the average weather of each month during periods which in most cases amounted to at least ten years. Except for about 400,000 in the United States, all the deaths occurred during the normal period immediately preced- ing the Great War. In addition to this, about 50,000,000 deaths in other countries were analyzed less intensively, but with es- sentially the same results. On the other hand, about 700,000 deaths in New York from 1877 to 1888 have been very minutely analyzed according to the day of death. For the six years from 1883 to 1888 the number of deaths each day has been compared with the weather day by day during the two weeks ending with the day of death. In another investigation 7200 deaths from lobar pneumonia in New York in 1913 were compared with the CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 21 weather for each day. Again, in the two largest hospitals in Boston the relation of the weather to 2300 deaths succeeding operations was looked into. At a later date the death rate during the influenza epidemic of 1918 in the United States was analyzed by still a different method in order to determine whether the weather had any effect in altering the rate from city to city. The net results of these investigations, as shown in World Power and Evolution and in various technical articles agree closely with those of the investigation of factory workers and students. They confirm the work of other investigators in show- ing almost beyond question that there is a distinct optimum condition of climate for man just as for plants and animals. This optimum varies relatively little from one set of people to another or from place to place. Even for negroes the departure from the white standard is by no means so large as would be expected, though it is unmistakable. Any departure from the optimum for a given race or individual seems to render people not only less efficient, but also more susceptible to disease and hence an easier prey to bacteria and other parasites. Moreover, as appears in World Power and Evolution, there is some evidence that departures from the optimum climate render people less buoyant, less capable of prolonged and steady mental activity, and correspondingly less likely to make progress. The signifi- cant fact about the whole matter is that, so far as I am aware, in every case where large bodies of people have been carefully analyzed, the same major responses to climate become evident, even though there may be differences in details. Thus the prog- ress of the last ten years seems to add appreciably to the general certainty as to the nature and importance of climatic optima, and as to the effect of departures therefrom upon health, effi- ciency, and progress. The difference between the present and first editions of this book in respect to natural selection and racial inheritance is especially important. It may be illustrated by ancient Greece. 22 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE In the first edition I supposed that the reason for the peculiar ability of Greece was a mystery which there was no immediate prospect of solving. It was the result of some unexplained bio- logical process. I believed that no matter where those particular people might have gone or at what period they had happened to live they would have achieved much more than any ordinary people. As events actually shaped themselves, the ancient Greeks migrated to a land near more ancient centers of civilization whence they could receive the inspiration of the greatest pre- ceding cultures. By reason of the numerous gulfs and harbors of their land the Greeks were easily able to get what they wanted from other countries, provided they had energy enough to sail abroad for material riches, and capacity enough to absorb the mental riches with which they came in contact. So far as climate is concerned, Greece appears to have enjoyed unusually favor- able conditions throughout most of the period from perhaps 1000 to 300 B.C., and especially about 400 B.C. Previous to 1100 B.C., however, there seems to have been an unfavorable period culminating perhaps 1300 to 1200 years before Christ, while at a later date a period of rapid climatic degeneration from 800 to 200 B.C. was followed by highly unfavorable conditions during the succeeding century. The favorable climate during the period of the greatest Grecian development apparently ren- dered the economic conditions distinctly more favorable than those of today, and stimulated the Greeks to a high degree of physical and mental energy. At the same time it rendered the environment unfavorable not only to the anopheles mosquito which causes malaria, but to other disease-bearing organisms. Thus the Greeks with their high inheritance enjoyed an environ- ment which gave full opportunity for the development of the best that was in them. The fall of Greece, according to the hy- pothesis set forth in this book ten years ago, was greatly in- fluenced by a rapid deterioration of climate. In the third centurv B.C., a decrease in rainfall caused a most serious diminu- CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 23 tion in the capacity of Greece to support population. This in- creased the political difficulties while at the same time the ability of the people to cope with such difficulties was diminished by a decline in the stimulating qualities of the climate. At the same time the increase in marshes and in stagnant pools, because of changed conditions of rainfall, made the environment favorable for malarial mosquitoes, while poverty, a poor food supply, and other adverse conditions also fostered disease. In general, the poor economic and political situation, and the unfavorable con- ditions of health tended to kill off the blonde Norse invaders who seem to have been the leaders among the early Greeks. At present my view of the rise and fall of Greece differs from the one set forth above in only one respect, but that is highly important. Instead of recognizing natural selection as an im- portant cause merely of the decline of Greece, I now regard it as one of the main causes of the rise of that country. The people who made Greece great, as I have shown in detail in The Char- acter of Races, were primarily the Dorians and especially the lonians. These people appear to have originated far to the north, perhaps in the great plains of Russia. They carne to Greece in a series of migrations, the first of which may have been that of the Achseans in the fourteenth century before Christ, while a later invasion culminated in the Dorian invasion two or three centuries later. The lonians appear to have been largely Greeks of the old Achaean and Minoan upper classes, who were led to migrate by the prolonged disturbances and fighting which harassed Greece for generations and perhaps centuries after the Dorian invasion. Attica had previously been almost depopu- lated and did not attract the Dorians because it was infertile and poverty-stricken by reason of its dryness. Later, however, perhaps in part because of an amelioration of climate, it became the refuge of large numbers of the old upper classes from the rest of Greece. When its population became too dense, some of the settlers, together with many of the more able and energetic 24 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE people from other parts of Greece, went across the Sea eastward to the Ionian coast of Asia Minor. Now we are expressly told by Thucydides and others not only that the settlers in Attica were a highly selected group from the leading families of the rest of Greece, but that they strenuously kept themselves apart from the lower classes. Even if an Athenian married a woman of the lower classes his children followed the social status of the mother and not of the father. Thus among the citizens of Athens a selected inheritance was kept almost unimpaired for many centuries. But note another important fact. Not only was there a rigid selection of good material when the lonians settled in Athens, but there appears to have been a perhaps more rigorous selec- tion during the earlier migrations of the people who finally be- came Greeks. Every migration is more or less selective because the weak, the feeble, the cowardly, and those lacking the spirit of adventure, together with those who lack determination, are gradually weeded out. The longer and more difficult the migra- tion, the more strenuous is the selection, especially among women and children. All but the most vigorous, both mentally and physically, are weeded out with peculiar rapidity. Thus the net result of practically every long or difficult migration in which women as well as men take part is to pick out a relatively small group of people of unusual capacity. The group may contain only one out of twenty, or one out of one hundred or a thousand of the original stock. Now the Athenians, as we have just seen, went through this selection twice, and may have gone through it at earlier times also. They also maintained the quality of the original stock more or less completely by refraining from inter- marriage with any but the elite of other regions and by various practices such as infanticide, which eliminated weaklings. Such selection with various modifications has been, I believe, one of the most effective factors in producing competent races. But what has this to do with climate? Much, because a large CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 25 number of the migrations of history appear to have been more or less directly started by climatic vicissitudes. For example, the Irish migration to America during the nineteenth century was due largely to the fact that a series of good potato harvests in the early decades of the century permitted the population of Ireland to increase enormously. Then came a series of unduly rainy years accompanied by bad crops. Famine and distress en- sued, and immediately an enormous migration to America set in. In later years, as is shown by Bruckner's data which I have cited in an article on A Neglected Factor in Race Development (Journal of Race Development, October, 1915), the migrations from Ireland and likewise from Germany to the United States have varied in close harmony with the climatic conditions and consequent agricultural prosperity of both the Old World and the New. When the crops are bad in Europe but good in Amer- ica, as is not uncommon, emigration from the Old World is greatly stimulated for a few years. On the contrary a period of bad crops in America coinciding with good crops in Europe gradually diminishes the pressure which leads to migration. Similar examples on a large scale have occurred time after time in the history of China, Central Asia, and other regions. Contrary to the common belief, most parts of the world normally contain practically as large a population as they are capable of holding under the social and economic conditions which happen to prevail at any given time and place. The birth rate among mankind, as Carr-Saunders convincingly shows in The Population Problem, is so large and responds so quickly to economic changes that only a few decades or generations are re- quired for even a relatively depopulated country to fill up to what Woodruff in his Expansion of Races has called the satura- tion point. Thus the great majority of migrations can be under- stood only by thinking of all parts of the world as normally having nearly their full quota of population. Among primitive hunting tribes, for example, this quota may mean only one 26 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE person per square mile, while in a modern highly civilized com- munity it may mean a thousand per square mile. The conditions are like those of a bucket filled to the brim with water. As soon as any object is dropped into the bucket some water runs over. In the same way, in most parts of the world any deterioration of economic conditions such as frequently arises from bad crops, immediately necessitates either a lowering of the standard of living or a diminution of population through an increased death rate by reason of poverty, famine, or war, or through migration. A migration, as a rule, is merely a slow drifting of people with unusual energy and initiativefrom unfavorable to favorable districts. But not infrequently, and especially after one of the sudden climatic vicissitudes which have been common in history, it becomes a violent stream of invasion such as many of the bar- barian outpourings about 1400 to 1200 years before Christ and again at various later periods. The original Greeks apparently took part in extensive wan- derings of both the mild and violent kind, and natural selection presumably worked among them effectively. The net result was a highly selected race which gave the world a marvelous group of famous men. These men, it appears, accomplished great things, not only because of their high innate qualities, but because grow- ingly favorable climatic conditions from six to four hundred years before Christ improved their economic situation and helped to give them great energy and splendid health. According to this view, Greece apparently lost her ability partly because the fine original stock became mingled with weaker elements, and partly because the change of climate which began soon after 400 B.C. and which took place most rapidly from 300 to 200 B.C., not only enabled malaria to spread with baleful rapidity, but introduced a stage of diminishing resources, scanty food supply, overpopulation, and lessened climatic stimulus. These factors presumably combined with other well-known conditions to produce poor health and perhaps restriction of families among CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 27 the upper classes, thus killing off the old dominant stock of northern origin. Hence the racial elements to which Greece owed her greatness disappeared, and the country fell into intellectual insignificance. Other factors undoubtedly cooperated, but such conditions as political corruption, social degeneracy, and undue personal ambition are probably results of racial decay and poor health quite as much as causes. The essential point is that at the beginning a time of climatic stress among nomads in rela- tively cool grasslands seems to have led to migration and a favorable type of selection. Another period of similar stress amid a highly developed people in a relatively southern land where there was less opportunity or incentive for migration led to the dying off of the more able people. Yet even in this case many of the most competent Greeks migrated, thus still farther impoverishing the mother country and giving rise to highly gifted colonies like the Alexandrian community in Egypt. To sum up the whole hypothesis of the relation of climate to civilization, here are the factors as I see them at present. Most parts of the world are so well populated that any adverse eco- nomic change tends to cause distress, disease, and a high death rate ; migration ensues among the more energetic and adventur- ous people. Perhaps the commonest cause of economic distress is variations in weather or climate which lead to bad crops or to dearth of grass and water for animals. Such economic distress almost inevitably leads to political disturbances and this again is a potent cause of migrations. The people who migrate perforce expose themselves to hardships and their numbers diminish until only a selected group of unusually high quality remains. Such people, either as warlike invaders or in small bands, enter a new country. They may find it well populated and merely impose themselves as a new ruling class, as seems to have happened several times in India, or they may find it depleted of people as in Attica. When the period of climatic stress is ended and the climate improves, the dominant newcomers not only possess an 28 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE unusually strong inheritance, but are stimulated by unusually good economic conditions and by improved conditions of health and energy. Moreover since the population is apt to remain below the saturation point so long as the climate improves, the standards of living tend to rise and to become relatively high. Thus many people are freed from the mere necessity of making a living and have the opportunity to devote themselves to the development of new ideas in literature, art, science, politics, and other lines of progress. The repeated coincidence between periods of improving climate and periods of cultural progress appears to be due not only to the direct stimulus of climate, as I sup- posed in the first edition of this book, but to that stimulus com- bined with a high racial inheritance due to natural selection. This, I am well aware, by no means offers a complete explanation of history, for many other elements must also be considered. But it helps to explain many historic events which have hitherto been only partially understood. Here, once more, is the sequence climatic changes produce economic results through an increase or a diminution of the food supply. Thus there arises either a temporary condition of under- population with comparative political tranquillity and oppor- tunities for the growth and expansion of civilization, or a con- dition of overpopulation with consequent political turmoil, war, migration, and the repression of civilization. This point of view was dominant when I wrote The Pulse of Asia and Palestine and Its Transformation. Climatic changes also appear to have a direct effect in stimulating or repressing man's physi- cal activity, a viewpoint which dominated the first edition of the present book. It is obvious that through their effect upon food, insects, bacteria, and man's own powers of resistance climatic changes must exert a great influence upon disease. Hence I was led to write World Power and Evolution from the standpoint of disease and the death rate. But this does not end the matter, for climate apparently exerts a direct selective CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 29 effect in preserving certain types of people and destroying others, and it certainly exerts these effects indirectly through various conditions already mentioned. Therefore it was only logical that The Character of Races should center around natural selection, especially in its climatic aspects. The next step is obviously a study of the relation of climate to mutations and thus to the origin of the new types among which natural selection makes its choice, but that is as yet impossible. Beyond this lies the synthesis of the effects which climate produces through economic and political conditions, through war and migration, food and natural resources, energy and health, and through natural selection and mutations. And all these results of the climatic environment must be put into due relation not only with the results of the other factors of physical environment, but with the opposite side of the shield, that is, with the purely human factors such as institutions, customs, ideas, and all man's passions, ideals, and aspirations. Then it will be possible to form a true philosophy of history. Meanwhile the present edition of Civilization and Climate tries to take a broader and deeper view of human progress than its predecessor, but it makes no claim to deal exhaustively with more than one small phase of the matter, namely, the direct effects of climate upon human ^health and energy. CHAPTER 11 RACE OR PLACE THE problem which confronts us is primarily to separate the direct effects of climate from those of inheritance, regard- less of whether the inheritance has been influenced by the climate of the past. It may be made concrete by comparing two sharply contrasted races, Teutons and negroes. Suppose that there were two uninhabited Egypts, exactly alike, and that one could be filled with negroes and the other with Teutons. Suppose that these settlers were average members of their races, and were equipped with the same religion, education, government, social institutions, and inventions. This might easily happen if the negroes came from the United States. Suppose, further, that neither race received new settlers from without, or lost any except through natural selection. Which would succeed best? "The Teutons, of course," is the answer. "What a foolish ques- tion." But is it so foolish? You are thinking of the first genera- tions. I am thinking of the twentieth or later. Does anyone know what five hundred or a thousand years of life in Egypt would do for either Teutons or negroes if no new blood were intro- duced ? At the end of that time the two sets of people would assuredly be different, for the effect of a diverse inheritance would last indefinitely. The advantage in this respect would presumably be on the side of the Teutons. I wish to emphasize this matter, for I shall have much to say about the effect of climate, and I want to make it perfectly clear that I do not underrate the impor- tance of race. Although the matter is by no means settled, many RACE OR PLACE 31 authorities think that the brain of the white man is more com- plex than that of his black brother. Strong, in the Pedagogical Seminary for 1913, and Morse, in the Popular Science Monthly for 1914, have shown that in Columbia, South Carolina, the white children are mentally more advanced than the colored. By applying the Binet tests to 225 children in two white schools and to 125 children in a colored school, they obtained the following table, showing the amount by which the two races exceeded or fell short of what would be expected. COLORED WHITE More than one year backward, 29.4% 10.2% Satisfactory, 69.8% 84.4% More than one year advanced, 0.8% 5.3% Among the white children those from the middle classes made a better showing than those of factory operatives, but both were ahead of the colored. So far as home environment is con- cerned, the factory children have almost no advantage over the colored children. A slight advantage may possibly arise from the fact that when the Binet tests were originally devised, they were designed to measure the capacities of white children. The negro race may have capacities which the white does not possess and which do not play a part in the tests. In appreciation of humor, for example, and in equability of temperament there can be little question that the black rnan surpasses the white. These things, however, can scarcely account for the fact that 29.4 per cent of the colored children showed a mental develop- ment more than a year behind that which would be expected from their age, while only 10.2 per cent of the white children were equally backward. So far as I am aware, every exact test which has been made on a large scale indicates mental superiority on the part of the white race, even when the two races have equal opportunities. For example, in Washington the colored children remain in 32 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE school quite as long as the white, but they do not accomplish so much in the way of study and do not reach so high a grade. In the cities of the South, Mayo and Loram find that where the races are given essentially the same instruction, the proportion of whites who are promoted is greater than that of negroes. Moreover, the difference seems to increase with years, which suggests that the average colored child not only stands below the average white child in mental development at all ages, but ceases to develop at an earlier age. In the high schools of New York, the superiority of the white race is shown by Mayo's examination of the average marks. By the time the children reach the high school, the processes of promotion have weeded out a much larger proportion of colored children than of white. Hence, the negroes form a specially selected group whose supe- riority to the average of their race is more marked than the superiority of the white high school children when compared with the rest of the white race. Nevertheless, the average marks of the white children are distinctly higher than those of the colored. In order to test the capacity of the two races in a wholly different way, I have made a comparison of white and colored workmen employed under precisely similar conditions. The first case was a cigar factory at Jacksonville, Florida. The employees were practically all Cubans. Both the whites and the blacks have very little education, and their home environment in Cuba differs to only the smallest extent. They earn good wages, but are often out of work, and are generally shiftless and unreliable. There is, of course, no color line in Cuba, and the same is true in the cigar factories. Black men and white work side by side at the same tables. In such a factory, if the black man is as capable as the white he has exactly as good a chance, for he is paid by the piece, and his earnings depend entirely on himself. What, then, do we find? Taking all the operatives, we have 39 white and 65 negroes. Their average earnings, as measured by the wages of two weeks, RACE OR PLACE 33 are in the ratio of 100 for the whites to only 51 for the negroes. To make the comparison more favorable to the negroes, let us eliminate those who roll low-grade cigars where little skill is required and the pay is low. We then have 39 white men and 44 negroes. They are doing exactly the same work under exactly the same conditions, but the whites earn a dollar where the negroes earn 75 cents. At a similar factory at Tampa, Florida, 17 colored men were at work and 303 white. In this case prac- tically all of the few negroes happened to be men of long experi- ence, while many of the whites were comparatively new. Never- theless, the whites are still on a par with the colored men, the ratio being 100 to 99.8. One of the best places for comparing the two races is the Bahama Islands. For reasons which I shall present later, the process of making "poor whites" has probably gone farther in the Bahamas than in almost any other Anglo-Saxon community. Part of the white people are like their race in other regions, but a large portion have unmistakably degenerated. Witness their intense and bigoted speech, their sunken cheeks and eyes, their sallow complexion, and their inert way of working. In spite of racial prejudice, there is no real color line in the Bahamas. Persons with more or less negro blood are worthy occupants of the highest positions, and are universally accepted in the most exclusive social circles. The British government gives the negro every possible opportunity. The state of affairs may be judged from the remarks of a "poor white" sailor, who said to me : "You want to know why I likes the southern states better than the North. It's because they hates a nigger and I hates him, too. What kind of a place is this where they do everything for the nigger and nothing for the white man? It's bad enough to have to go to jail, but it's damned hard for a white man to be taken there by a nigger constable." In one Bahaman village I saw negro girls teaching white children in the public schools. In that same village a number of the leading white men cannot read or 34 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE write. When they were children their parents would not send them to school with negroes. The despised negroes learned to read and write, but have now largely forgotten those accomplish- ments. The proud whites grew up in abject ignorance. Today the same thing is going on. I visited two villages where the white children are staying away from school because they will not go to negro teachers. The homes of such whites are scarcely better than those of their colored neighbors, and their fathers are called "Jim" and "Jack" by the black men with whom they work. Racial prejudice apparently works more harm to the whites than the blacks. So far as occupations go there is no difference, for all alike till the soil, sail boats, and gather sponges. When the lumber industry was introduced into the islands, whites and blacks were equally ignorant of the various kinds of work involved in cutting trees and converting them into lumber. The managers did not care who did the work so long as it was done. They wanted three things : strength, docility or faithful- ness, and brains. They soon found that in the first two the negroes were superior. Time and again persons in authority, chiefly Americans, but also some of the more capable native whites, told me that if they wanted a crew of men to load a boat or some such thing, they would prefer negroes every time. The poor white shirks more than the colored man, he is not so strong, and he is proud and touchy. Other things being equal, the negro receives the preference. But other things are not equal. The very men who praise the negroes generally added : "But you can't use a negro for everything. They can't seem to learn some things, and they don't know how to boss a job." The payroll reflects this. Even though the negroes receive the preference, the 4*00 who are employed earn on an average only about 60 per cent as much as the 57 white men. If we take only the 57 most competent negroes, their average daily wages are still only 88 per cent as great as those of the native whites. The difference is purely a matter of brains. Although the white man may be ignorant and RACE OR PLACE 35 inefficient, with no more training than the negro, and although his father and grandfather were scarcely better, he possesses an inheritance of mental quickness and initiative which comes into evidence at the first opportunity. All these considerations seem to point to an ineradicable racial difference in mentality. As the plum differs from the apple not only in outward form and color, but in inward flavor, so the negro seems to differ from the white man not only in feature and complexion, but in the workings of the mind. No amount of training can eradicate the difference. Cultivation may give us superb plums, but they will never take the place of apples. We have tried to convert the black man into an inferior white man, but it cannot be done. Initiative, inventiveness, versatility, and the power of leadership are the qualities which give flavor to the Teutonic race. Good humor, patience, loyalty, and the power of self-sacrifice give flavor to the negro. With proper training he can accomplish wonders. No one can go to a place like Hampton Institute without feeling that there is almost no limit to what may be achieved by cultivation. In an orderly, quiet way, those negro boys and girls go about their daily tasks and give one the feeling that they are making a real contribution to the world's welfare. To be sure, they work slowly, they are not brilliant in their classes, they rarely have new ideas in their manual work, but yet they are faithful. The willing, happy spirit of their work is something that we nervous, worried white people need sorely to learn. Once in a long time there comes a leader, a man to whom both white and black look up, but such leadership is scarcely the genius of the race. Yet leadership is what the black man must have. At such places as Hampton he gets it, and one realizes that the white man's initiative joined to the Christian spirit which is there so dominant can give a training which overcomes much of the handicap of race. Having turned aside to pay tribute to the potency of race, education, and religion in determining the status of civilization, 36 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE let us come back to physical environment. What part does this play? Is it so important that a strong race in an unfavorable climate is likely to make no better showing than a weak race in a favorable climate? How far can a bad climate undo the effect of a good training? In answer to these questions, we may well compare the Teu- tonic and negro races when each is removed from the climate in which it originally developed. Before proceeding to this a word should be added to forestall any possible misunderstanding of rny attitude toward the southern parts of the United States and toward other progressive regions which, nevertheless, suffer somewhat from climatic handicaps. In searching for the truth I shall be forced to say some things which may not be wholly pleasing to residents of such regions. It must be clearly under- stood, however, that these are not stated on my own authority. All are based either on the consensus of opinion among a large number of persons including many southerners, or upon the exact figures of the United States census or other equally re- liable sources. My part has been simply to interpret them. Be- lieving that the South contains a great number of people who in all essential respects have an inheritance equal to that of the best northern stocks, I have tried to find out why the southern part of the United States has prospered less than the northern. This does not mean that I reject the old ideas as to the cause, but simply that I emphasize another which has not received sufficient consideration. It does not discredit the South nor its people. It does not alter the fact that southerners possess a courtesy and thoughtfulness which we of the worried and hurried North need greatly to imitate. Nor does it mean that men of genius are not as likely to be born in one section as another. Instead of this, it merely indicates that in addition to the many efforts now being made to foster progress in the South by other means, we should add a most vigorous attempt to discover ways of overcoming the handicap of climate. This book is written with RACE OR PLACE 37 the profound hope that the truth which it endeavors to discover may especially help those parts of the world whose climate, al- though favorable, does not afford the high degree of stimulation which in certain other restricted areas is so helpful. Let us first undertake a study of what the census shows as to negroes and whites in different parts of the United States. The only people whom we can compare with accuracy are the farmers, for they are the only ones for whom exact statistics are avail- able. Fortunately they are the part of the community where social prejudices and other hampering conditions have the smallest influence. The prosperity of the farmer, more than that of almost any other class of society, depends upon his own indi- vidual effort. If he is industrious, he need never fear that he and his family will not have a roof over their heads and something to eat. Even when the crops are bad, he rarely is in danger of suffering as factory operatives often suffer, at least not in the eastern United States, with which alone we are now concerned. Moreover, the prejudice against colored people has little effect upon farmers. No one hesitates to buy vegetables peddled by a darkey farmer. Finally, farming is the occupation in which the South has been least hampered as compared with the North. For over half a century the negro has been able to buy land freely in any part of the country. The southerners, whether white or black, have suffered economically because of slavery and the con- sequent war, but they have a good soil and a climate far better for agriculture than that of the North, and they have peculiarly good opportunities to raise tobacco and cotton, two of the great- est money-making crops in the world. Taken all in all, the farmers of the country ought to show the relative capacities of different races and of the same race under different conditions better than almost any other class of people. In 1904 the United States Census Bureau published a bulle- tin on the negro. From that I have prepared Table 1 showing the relative conditions in four groups of states in 1900. The first s E & S3 B EC 4) o * 2 | * I w r tj w o eg Q - B S I a i x x X wi Q o X X i "a ^ I !f s a si J3.3 o .-3 2 ^C I .S * t yi Op > ^-s -< to .o c: , -H o CO >p CO > ^8 3 . .^ o> e~oi ^.co ^S W&fc -" AQ CTs O #- T* -* CO ^-r-1 ^O^-^ ^-O ^ , - ^. i O *O O *O O O> O *O O O Q i < (. .f e> - o o c& o & o _r o ^ < ^^^^^^ M -S^ 1 ' O 5C O 3 fl -.3" s|l|| i^o ii ^ ^^ A a *H 0) bO^ "^ O bD as C 2 ^ ^ p 2 Total numb Average acr Average a per farm, land whic RACE OR PLACE 39 row of numbers (line 1) shows the total number of white and colored farmers. The second row shows that the farms of the northern white men average about 100 acres in size, while those of the southern white men are larger. The colored farms, on the other hand, have an average size of about 50 acres. In the next row of figures, line 3, we notice that the northerners forge ahead. Even in the relatively hilly states of New York and Pennsyl- vania, the white farmers have improved 63.5 acres per farm, or 69 per cent of the whole, leaving only 31 per cent in the rough state of bushes or woods. The northern negroes do exactly as well in proportion to their holdings, for they have cleared 33.0 acres, which is also 69 per cent of the average farm. In the Carolinas, Georgia, and Florida, on the contrary, the white men have improved only 34 per cent of their land, and the colored men 58 per cent. For the states farther west (comparison B), approximately similar conditions prevail. The negroes are obliged to clear a larger percentage than the whites because their small holdings would not otherwise furnish a living. The significance of the figures lies in the fact that the northerners, whether white or black, show more energy in improving their land than do the southerners of the same kind. Since this table was in print the corresponding data for 1910 and 1920 have appeared. Unfortunately they are less full than for 1900 and do not include the value of products, line 9. For line 4 the percentages for each of the three census years are as follows : TABLE 2. Comparison A Comparison B N.W. N.N. S.W. S.N. N.W. N.N. S.W. S.N. 1900 100 59 28 11 100 49 32 13 1910 100 81 41 24 100 48 25 14 1920 100 74 64 20 100 41 31 16 In comparison A, the gain of the other groups in relation to the northern white farmers is noticeable. This, however, does 40 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE not mean merely that agriculture is improving in the South, but also that it is declining in the Middle Atlantic States. In B, the percentages are almost unchanged. In both comparisons of Table 1 items 4 and 9 are the most significant. They show the value of the farms and the value of the annual products. In each item of Table 1 the values are stated in dollars as given in the census report, while underneath I have added percentages. In computing the percentages the highest value is reckoned as 100 per cent and the rest figured accord- ingly. In each item of both comparisons for all three census years, as given in Table 2, the northern whites stand at the top. In general, taking both comparisons into account, the northern white man's farm is worth twice as much as that of his colored neighbor, and he gets twice as much from it. The southern white man has a farm worth less than that of the northern negro, but he gets from it approximately the same amount of products. The southern negro's farm is worth less than half as much as the southern white man's and he gets from it about two thirds as much. Taking all the farms from our four groups of states and reckoning them according to the value of what they actually produced in 1900 and of their value in 1920, the census ranks them as shown in Table 3. TABLE 3. THE RELATIVE EFFICIENCY OF WHITE AND NEGRO FARMERS IN THE NORTH AND SOUTH Per value of Per value of products in 1900 farms in 1920 Northern whites, 100 100 Southern whites, 51 44 Northern negroes, 49 49 Southern negroes, 34 18 This little table possesses profound significance. It shows un- mistakably two types of contrast. First, there is the racial con- RACE OR PLACE 41 trast, the result of long inheritance. That, apparently, is what makes the negroes fall below the whites in both the North and the South. There is also a climatic contrast. That, apparently, is why the negroes who come to the North rise above the usual level of their race, while the whites of the South fall below the level of theirs. I realize that the contrast between the two sec- tions is explained in a hundred ways by as many different people. One ascribes it to the fact that slavery was a poor system eco- nomically. Another says that the South is cursed for having consented to the sin of slavery. Again, we are told that the pre- dicament of the South is due to the War of Secession, the failure to develop manufactures, the absence of roads and railroads, bad methods of farming, the presence of the negro making the white man despise labor, and many other equally important causes which cannot here be named. Still other authorities ascribe the condition of the South to its supposed settlement by adventurers, whereas the North had its Pilgrims. I would not minimize the importance of these factors. All are of real significance, and if any had been different, the South would not be quite what it is. All depend upon the two funda- mental conditions of race or inheritance, and place or climate. Yet in the contrast between the North and South, the climatic effects seem to be the more potent. Slavery failed to flourish in the North not because of any moral objection to it, for the most godly Puritans held slaves, but because the climate made it un- profitable. In a climate where the white man was tremendously energetic and where a living could be procured only by hard and unremitting work, it did not pay to keep slaves, for the labor of such incompetent people scarcely sufficed to provide even them- selves with a living, and left little profit for their masters. In the South slavery was profitable because even the work of an ineffi- cient negro more than sufficed to produce enough to support him. Moreover, the white man was not energetic, and his manual work was not of much more value than that of a negro. Hence, it was 42 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE easy to fall into the habit of using his superior brain, and letting the black man perform the physical labor. If the Puritans had settled in Georgia, it is probable that they would have become proud slave-holders, despising manual work. So far as inheritance is concerned, the white southerners, according to the generally accepted principles of biology, must be essentially as well off as the white men of the North. New England has probably had a certain advantage from the strong fiber of her early settlers, but that section is excluded from our comparison because it has so few colored farmers. In New York, Pennsylvania, New Jersey, and the states farther west, the white farmers in 1900 were of highly mixed origin, and there is little reason to think that they inherit any greater capacity than do the white men of the South. Hence, we infer that the difference shown by the census is largely a matter of climate. It has arisen partly by indirect means such as slavery and disease, partly by direct means such as the disinclination to physical exertion. This demands emphasis, for we are told that the South needs nothing but a fair opportunity, plenty of capital, and abundant roads, railroads, and factories, or else it needs only education, a new respect of one race for the other, cooperation between the two for the sake of the common good, and a deeper application of the principles of Christ. All these things are sadly needed, but it is doubtful whether they can work their full effect unless sup- plemented by a new knowledge of how to neutralize the climatic influences which seem to underlie so many southern problems. In the climate of the South a part of the white population becomes a prey to malaria, the hookworm, and other debilitating ail- ments. People cease to be careful about food and sanitation. Even those who are in good health do not feel the eager zest for work which is so notable in the parts of the world where the climatic stimulus is at a maximum. Thus one thing joins with another to cause a part of the people to fall far below the level of their race, and to become "Poor Whites," or "Crackers." RACE OR PLACE 43 These increase in number as one passes from a more to a less favorable climate. It is their run-down, unkempt farms which bring the average of the southern whites so dangerously near the level of the negroes. The best farms of the South vie with those of the North. They show what could be done if all the inhabitants could be instilled with the energy and wisdom of the best. Aside from North America the only large area where Teutons and negroes come into direct contact as permanent inhabitants is South Africa. There they meet on practically equal terms. The English and Boers began to settle in South Africa in large num- bers only in the first half of the nineteenth century. In 1921 the South African Republic contained about 1,500,000 Europeans, 4,700,000 Bantu natives, and 700,000 other natives and Asiatics. A large proportion of the white men were not born there, and hence the new conditions have not had time to pro- duce their full effect. The majority of the natives are Zulus, but the most capable appear to be the Basutos, an allied race who have preserved a large measure of independence in the Draken- berg mountains. Both the Zulus and the Basutos came from the North a few generations ago. Some preceded the white man and some have come since his arrival. The colored people are most numerous in the north and east of the Republic, that is, in Rhodesia and Natal. The white men are most abundant in the south and in the central plateau, that is, in Cape Colony, Orange River Colony, and Transvaal. With ever increasing force, however, the blacks are pushing into the white man's country. They are brought as laborers for the mines ; they are wanted for the farms ; they are in demand as servants ; and they are themselves taking up farms and success- fully cultivating them. They are doing more than this, however, for they are actually ousting the Europeans. In 1902 the Eng- lish and the Boers finished a bitter war. Ten years later their enmity had almost vanished in the common fear of the negro. Aside from the disturbances due to the European War of 1914, *4 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the great political question has long been the black man. One party advocates segregation, with a white man's South Africa in the highlands from Transvaal southward, and a black man's South Africa in Natal and Rhodesia. No black man, they say, should be allowed to live permanently outside his own country, although he might go elsewhere to work temporarily. The other party holds that such measures are too radical, but it also recognizes the gravity of the situation. The problem presents itself under an economic guise. The colored men have a lower standard of living than the whites. Hence they work more cheaply. They furnish so abundant a supply of labor that white laborers have no chance. Thus a large number of the Europeans even a tenth according to ardent believers in the future of South Africa are "poor whites." They are a shiftless set, living from hand to mouth, proud of their race, yet less efficient than the blacks. The prob- lem of preventing them from becoming an immediate charge upon the community is serious. They lack the push and energy which characterize the rest of the white population. According to Stevens, in his book White and Black, 5 per cent of the white population in certain regions have fallen so low that they would rather resort to crime than work in competition with the black man. These figures have been questioned, but they are abundantly confirmed by Dr. Andrew Balfour, Director-in- Chief of the Wellcome Bureau of Scientific Research in London. In some lectures on Sojourners in the Tropics and Problems of Acclimatisation, published in The Lancet in 1923, he states that he "referred the matter to Colonel P. G. Stock, of the Ministry of Health, who knows South Africa intimately, and he confirmed Huntington's statement, pointing out, however, that in parts of the Transvaal chronic malaria may be to blame." The most sinister fact is that these "poor whites" appear to have been largely born in the country. The newcomers are on the whole more energetic. They find employment, and if they have difficulty RACE OR PLACE 45 in one place, move on to another. The poor whites lack the ini- tiative to do this. If they fall into difficulties, they tend to lie down and give up. They need higher wages than the blacks in order to maintain their traditional standard of living. They are not efficient enough to get higher wages. If they had the restless energy which characterizes the children and grandchildren of emigrants from Europe in Canada, for example, they would scarcely fall into such straits. Since the problem is economic, the South Africans are striv- ing to apply economic remedies. This is wise, but success is doubtful unless other factors are also considered. Back of the economic facts, and in many ways conditioning them, lies the climate. South Africa is supposed to have a climate admirably adapted to Europeans. I shared the common opinion until I began to gather statistics of the eff ect of climate upon efficiency. These, as will be shown later, indicate that although the South African climate is pleasant, it lacks the stimulating qualities which are so important in Europe and North America. This lack of stimulus increases rapidly as one goes from south to north. Here, then, is the situation that confronts us : In South Africa the white men settled first in the regions most favorable from a climatic point of view and then pushed northward into worse conditions. Even the best parts of South Africa cannot approach England and Holland in the excellence of their cli- mate. Hence, the white settlers are everywhere at a disadvantage. On the other hand, the Bantu negroes have come into South Africa from the north, where the climate is far less favorable than in their new homes. Thus the two races face each other under conditions which lessen the white man's energy, while they stimulate the black man. The whites are still far ahead, and will doubtless continue to be so indefinitely. Nevertheless, the weaker ones are being weeded out and prepared for destruction. What the final result will be, no man can say. It depends upon whether we can discover a means of preventing the deterioration which 46 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE now seems to attack a portion of the population when people move from a good climate to a worse. A more striking case than that of South Africa is found in the Bahama Islands. At the time of the American Revolution a considerable number of Loyalists were so faithful to England that they sacrificed their all in order to escape from the new flag with its stars and stripes. Leaving their homes in Georgia and other southern states they sought the British territory of the Bahamas. Other colonists came from Great Britain. Now, after from three to five generations, the new environment has had more opportunity than in South Africa to produce its full effect. Almost nowhere else in all the world have people of the English race lived as genuine colonists for several generations in so tropical a climate. What has been the result? There can be but one answer. It has been disastrous. Compare the Bahamas with Canada. The same sort of people went to both places. Today the descendants of the Loyalists in Canada are one of the strongest elements in causing that country to be conspicu- ously well governed and law-abiding, and the descendants of other colonists, both British and French, vie with them in this matter. In the Bahamas the descendants of the same type of people show today a larger proportion of poor whites than can probably be found in any other Anglo-Saxon community. Al- though no figures are available, my own observations lead to the conclusion that the average white farmer is scarcely ahead of the average negro. Whatever the exact figures may be, there can be no question that in the Bahamas the two races tend to approach the same level. This seems to indicate a marked retrogression of the white race in regions which are climatically unsuitable. Let me hasten to say that many of the more intelligent Bahamans do not differ from the corresponding portions of the Anglo-Saxon race else- where. At home they feel themselves handicapped, but when the young people go away to the northern United States or Eng- RACE OR PLACE 47 land, they frequently show marked ability. Their inheritance is still good. As to the poor whites, who were described in connec- tion with the lumber industry, it is not so certain that their in- heritance remains unimpaired, for in some villages genuine ab- normalities both of body and mind are seen. This, however, may be due to the intermarriage of cousins which has been common in certain communities. The inefficiency of many of the white Bahamans, however, is not due to intermarriage, as is sometimes implied, for villages where this prevails are scarcely worse than those where it is no more common than in America. Nor is the inefficiency due to disease. The hookworm is practically unknown. According to a report of Dr. McHattie, Chief Medical Officer of the Islands, only two cases had been reported up to October, 1913. In this report, for which I am indebted to the courtesy of Dr. J. A. Ferrell of the Rockefeller International Health Commission, the author points out that "the remarkably rapid manner in which the soil . . . dries after even the heaviest rain" prevents the development of the infective larvae. For similar reasons malaria is no more prevalent than in Delaware, for instance, and in gen- eral the islands are decidedly healthful. A monotonous diet may be another detrimental factor, but it is scarcely the root of the matter. Many of the people are well fed, and all could be so if they displayed any energy. Indeed, many people say that life is altogether too easy in the Bahamas. The soil is wonderfully fertile, crops of some kind will ripen at all seasons, and a man can work less than half his time and still readily procure an abundance to eat and wear for himself and his family. On the other hand, we are often told that the difficulties of life have broken the spirit of the inhabitants. The soil, in spite of its rich- ness, is thin, and rocks are so abundant that the plough is almost unknown. Hand agriculture in little patches in the midst of naked limestone is the rule. It cannot be denied that there are difficulties in comparison with many other tropical countries. 48 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE For instance, I was talking with a negro whose parents were in a slave ship bound from Africa to Cuba when a British warship captured it. The slaves were taken to the Bahamas and liberated. In answer to a question as to how his parents liked the islands compared with Africa, the son said: "They didn't like it. They used to say, 'In Africa one could lie around all day and do nothing and always find something to eat. Here one has to work or else starve.* " The truth seems to be that compared with North Prussia or Maine the Bahamas are a very easy place in which to make a living, but that much more work is needed than in some other tropical regions. They are at the happy mean. Other difficulties such as the tropical hurricanes which sweep over the country once in every few years ; insect pests, which are neither more nor less harmful than in other countries ; the American tariff; competition with Cuba, and above all the iso- lated position of the islands are frequently cited as causes of the constant Bahaman failures. The islands are always suffering from bad luck, and something must be to blame. All these various factors doubtless play a part in retarding the development of the Bahamas. Back of them, however, lies a factor of even greater import, namely, an inertia due to the climate. It does not cause the difficulties mentioned above, but it aggravates them and makes it almost impossible to overcome them. I talked about this with perhaps fifty of the more intelli- gent people, including both natives and foreigners who have been there a number of years. Almost without exception they said the same thing. "This climate is one of the best in the world. You can see that for yourself. It is very healthful, and we have very few sicknesses. The only trouble is that it does not make one feel like work. In winter it is all right, although even then we cannot fly around the way you Americans do. We always feel lazy, and in summer we want to sit around all the time." As an American picturesquely put it : "Until I came to the Bahamas I never appreciated posts. Now I want to lean against every one RACE OR PLACE 49 that I see." Many of the men and almost all the women com- plained of feeling tired. Even the children are listless. One young man stated the case very strongly, "We go to bed tired in sum- mer and we get up more tired, and the summer lasts from April to October." Again and again people said : "Oh, it's all very well for you to think we're lazy, but try living here six months or a year and you'll be as lazy as we are. It's something in the air. Just look at these young ministers who come out from England. At first they are full of energy, but after a year or two it oozes out, though their spirit is still as zealous as ever." Two of the ministers spoke of the fact that when they came out they thought nothing of walking twenty miles, but now they dread the thought of two. Several of the most thoughtful and intelligent islanders, men who have succeeded in business and whose judgment would be respected anywhere, said: "We know that we are physically ttnable to do what English and Americans can do. We are weaker than our fathers, and they were weaker than theirs. It is a grief to send our children away, but in our hearts we know that this is not a white man's country." All this, it must be remembered, is not due to any specific disease, so far as we are aware. Indeed, I met several people who said that a stay of a few years in the Bahamas had improved their health, but at the same time had made them feel inefficient. Aside from extremely ignorant persons whose opinion is of little value, the only men who spoke of the climate more hope- fully were five or six highly trained officials and others occupy- ing positions of authority. These men, without exception, can control their own time. In most cases their office hours are from 9 or 10 a.m. to 2 p.m., or less. They are men of naturally strong physique ; they have the opportunity and the will to take regular exercise ; and, most important of all, they make long and fre- quent visits to the United States or England. The benefit to be derived from a visit to a more bracing cli- mate is astonishing. The contrast between the dull, sallow com- 50 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE plcxions and thin cheeks of the women and girls who have always lived on the islands and the round, rosy cheeks of those who have recently come back from a long stay at the North is most striking. According to a local saying you cannot tell whether a Bahaman woman is pretty until she goes away, and has a chance to fill out her cheeks and get some color. It is by no means strange that the stronger, more energetic young white people are fast leaving the islands. I asked a Bahaman girl, who had been study- ing nursing in New York, whether she enjoyed life more in the United States than in the Bahamas. "How can one help enjoying it more there?" she answered. "There one feels like doing things. Here one never feels like anything." Like almost everyone else she was sure that it was the climate even more than the new social environment which made the difference. One thing that surprised me was to hear the Bahamans speak of the stimulus of living in Florida. A native merchant remarked: "If I hire a new man I don't have to ask whether he has been to Florida. I know it by the way he works, but it docs not last long." Here again the social environment is an important factor, but various people told me that the air somehow makes them feel more capable of work in Florida than at home. The women of Florida I heard them say it themselves are pale and wan compared with their northern sisters. One of them, whose color still shows her northern origin, remarked: "When I come home after a summer in the North, I am full of energy and see all sorts of things that I want to change about the house. But after a month or two I don't care whether things are fixed or not." One hears the same sort of thing everywhere. A factory superin- tendent from Atlanta, Georgia, told me that the Florida work- men, even the most skillful mechanics, drive him frantic because they are so shiftless and so ready to take a day off whenever they feel like it far more so than at Atlanta, even though Atlanta seems slow to northerners. Yet, in spite of all these things, Florida is a more stimulating place than the Bahamas. RACE OR PLACE 51 Its summers are not much better, but its winters are sometimes frosty, while in the Bahamas the thermometer practically never goes below 50 F. Perhaps of greater importance, as we shall see later, is the fact that in Florida the temperature from day to day varies much more rapidly than in the Bahamas, even though both places are in the same latitude. Hence, the mainland is blessed with a genuine climatic stimulus compared with the uni- form islands. The last thing to be said about the Bahamas concerns the effect of the climate on mental activity. Practically all the islanders with whom I talked thought that the effect of the cli- mate on mental activity is at least as great as on physical. Several of the more thoughtful, without any suggestion on my part, put the matter in this way: "The worst thing about this climate is the effect on the mind. Not that people do not have as good minds as elsewhere, but one soon gets weary of hard mental effort. It is extremely difficult to concentrate one's thoughts. At night one cannot seem to make himself read anything serious nothing but the lightest kind of stories." In our own southern states one hears the same complaint. Even in Virginia the book- sellers say that during the long summer almost no one touches a serious book. One feels it everywhere, for on the trains, at the railroad stations, and at the newsdealers it is generally difficult to find the higher grade of magazines. Time and again during a recent journey of three months in the southern states I tried to get such papers as the Outlook, Independent, Harper's, At- lantic* Review of Reviews, The Century, and so forth but all that I could find was trashy story magazines. The dealers rarely keep the better magazines because people will not read them. Lack of training surely has something to do with the matter, but mental inertia due to lack of climatic stimulus seems to be at least equally important. Let us return now to our question as to a Teutonic and a negro Egypt. The farmers of the northern and southern states, 52 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the race problem of South Africa, and the backwardness of the Bahamas, all seem to point to the same conclusion. When the white man migrates to climates less stimulating than those of his original home, he appears to lose in both physical and mental energy. This leads to carelessness in matters of sanitation and food, and thus gives greater scope to the diseases which under any circumstances would find an easy prey in the weakened bodies. The combination of mental inertia and physical weakness makes it difficult to overcome the difficulties arising from isola- tion, from natural disasters, or from the presence of an inferior race, and this in turn leads to ignorance, prejudice, and idleness. Thus there arises a vicious circle which keeps on incessantly. From its revolving edge a part of the community is thrown off as poor whites, whose number increases in proportion to the ener- vating effect of the climate and the consequent speed with which the circle revolves. That climate is the original force which sets the wheel in motion seems to be evident, because it is only in adverse climates that we find the "cracker" type of "poor white trash" developing in appreciable numbers. If white men lived a thousand years in Egypt it seems probable that a large propor- tion of them would degenerate to this type. Whether they would still retain an inheritance of health and mentality sufficient to keep them ahead of a similar body of negroes can scarcely be determined. The chief reason for doubt in this respect is that we do not yet know just how natural selection would work in such a case. It would almost certainly act in two ways. First, many of the abler young people of the white race would presumably migrate, as they do in the Bahamas. This tendency is generally strongest in the upper classes who can afford to send their children away to school. It is also strong among the young people of all classes who possess more than the average initiative, ambition, and physical strength. It becomes weaker and weaker as one goes down the scale, and almost ceases among the "poor whites," who RACE OR PLACE 53 have so little mental capacity and so much physical inertia that in spite of much grumbling they remain where they are and compete with the colored people. The other kind of natural selection consists of a selective death rate. Children who inherit certain physical and mental traits are more likely to die than are children who do not possess those traits. What the traits are which cause extermination we do not know. A fair skin, a nervous temperament, an excess of activity, and an unwilling- ness or incapacity to get sufficient rest may be qualities which doom certain white stocks to gradual extinction outside their own climate. In places like South Africa and the Bahamas the temperament which is willing to intermarry with the colored people helps certain types of white people to perpetuate their inheritance, but at the same time it gradually eliminates the qualities of energy, initiative, and inventiveness which seem to be so much more characteristic of Nordics than of negroes. It must not be forgotten that theoretically it may be possible that some day a carefully controlled series of crosses between whites and blacks may eliminate the weak traits of each and combine the good traits. Thus a race may arise which resembles negroes in its good temper and its capacity to withstand a tropical climate, but which will have the progressive, executive, and inventive capacities of the white race. Such crosses have been made among animals. For example, Mr. M. F. C. Honore of the Transvaal has sent me the following quotation which he believes to be prophetic of what will some day happen in South Africa. It is from Winston Churchill, the British Cabinet Minis- ter. "At Naivasha [practically on the equator in British East Africa] there is the Government stock farm. One may see in their various flocks, the native sheep, the half-bred English, the three quarter bred, etc. The improvement is amazing. The native sheep is a hairy animal, looking to the unpractised eye more like a goat than a sheep. Crossed with Sussex or Australian blood, his descendant is transformed into a woollen beast of 54 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE familiar aspect. At the next cross the progeny is almost indis- tinguishable from the purebred English in appearance, but better adapted to the African sun and climate. It is the same with the cattle. In the first generation the hurnp of the African ox vanishes. In the second he emerges a respectable English shorthorn." Such carefully controlled crossbreeding may perhaps be possible among mankind after hundreds or thousands of years. But first we must know what human qualities are "unit char- acters" so that they are inherited according to the Mendelian law and are not due to the combination of a series of such char- acters. Then we must learn what qualities are dominant over others so that the presence of one hides the other. Another highly complex problem is to determine what qualities are linked with others so that one cannot be inherited without the other. The fact that linked qualities are very common may mean that cer- tain good qualities like the tolerance of the negro for a hot cli- mate can never be inherited without certain undesirable qualities like the lack of care for the future which is one of the chief causes of negro shiftlessness. Even if such linkage is not an insuperable barrier to the production of a really new race by intelligent crossbreeding there still remains the almost insurmountable obstacle of deep-seated human customs, racial antipathies, and modern ideas of individual liberty. Nevertheless, it is worth while to reflect on the following dream of Laf cadio Hearn : "It is neither unscientific nor unreasonable to suppose the world even- tually peopled by a race different from any now existing yet created by the blending of the best types of all races ; uniting western energy with far eastern patience, northern vigor with southern sensibility, the highest ethical feelings developed by all great religions with the largest mental faculties evolved by all civilizations ; speaking a single tongue composed from the richest and strongest elements of all preexisting human speech, and RACE OR PLACE 55 forming a society unimaginably superior yet unimaginably un- like to anything which now is or will ever be." This is an inspiring dream even though most biologists now regard it as impossible. So far as climate is concerned the hard reality seems to be that at present, both by its direct action and through natural selection a warm, monotonous, and unstimu- lating climate tends to reduce human activity both physical and mental, regardless of race. CHAPTER 111 THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS THUS far we have dealt with the temperate zone. Even the Bahamas lie north of the Tropic of Cancer. Let us now turn to the torrid zone, which contains the world's richest and most inviting fields of future development. Let us inquire into the effect of that region upon Europeans who attempt to live there permanently. The isolation of the tropical regions, their lack of facilities for transportation, and the great difficulties of agriculture will doubtless be overcome, but that will by no means solve the problem. Two great obstacles will still remain the native inhabitants and the white man's own mind and body. Whatever may be the cause, it is generally agreed that the native races within the tropics are dull in thought and slow in action. This is true not only of the African negroes, the South American Indians, and the people of the East Indies, but of the inhabitants of southern India and the Malay peninsula. Perhaps they will change, but the fact that the Indians both of Asia and South America have been influenced so little by from one to four hundred years of contact with the white man affords little ground for hope. Judging from the past, there is scant reason to think that their character is likely to change for many gen- erations. Until that time comes they will be one of the white man's greatest obstacles. Experience shows that the presence of an inferior race in large numbers tends constantly to lower the standards of the dominant race. Here in America, although the negro forms only a tenth part of the population, he is one of our gravest problems. Yet he is not so great a handicap as THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 57 are the native races of the tropics. Whatever the negro may have been when he was first brought to America, he is now less stolid and indifferent, more subject to stimulating influences than he was when he came, or than the Indians of tropical America. It is literally true in South America, for instance, that the more an Indian is paid the less he will work. If one day's pay will buy two days' food, he will work half the time ; if the pay is increased so that one day's pay will buy food for three days, he will work one third of the time. The experiment has been tried again and again. The most considerate employers of tropical labor agree with the most inconsiderate that in general it is use- less to attempt to spur the Indians by any motive beyond the actual demands of food and shelter. Kindness and consideration on the part of the employer undoubtedly promote faithfulness, but they seem rarely to arouse ambition or energy. With the negro in Africa, as everyone knows, much the same condition prevails, but where he has been brought to the United States this is by no means so true. For example, in Central America it is generally thought that a negro from Jamaica is more efficient than an Indian, while a negro from the United States is much more efficient. The negro in the United States is generally con- sidered more efficient than he was in Africa, whereas his stay-at- home brother and the Indian of tropical America, remaining in their old environment, do not seem to have changed. Doubtless the change in the negro is due to a new social en- vironment quite as much as to a new physical environment, and many authorities believe that the social change is the more im- portant. This, however, does not materially alter the case. As conditions are now, it is extremely difficult to change the physical environment of tropical races so long as they remain in their present habitat, and it seems to be equally difficult to change their social environment. Those who dwell permanently in the white man's cities are influenced somewhat, but the results are often disastrous. Here as almost everywhere within twenty de- 58 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE grees of the equator, the general tendency seems to be to revert to the original condition as soon as immediate contact with the white man is removed. This does not mean that contact with a higher civilization will never benefit the people of the tropics, but merely that the process is bound to be slow. The aborigines of tropical America, for example, show little sign either of disappearing, or being swallowed up by a multitude of immigrants, as has been the case in temperate latitudes, nor do they appear to be changing their character. On the contrary, in Latin America, the only tropical region except Australia where the white man has settled in large numbers, the proportion of Indian blood is apparently increas- ing at the expense of the white, and the Indians act and think almost like their ancestors three or four centuries ago. This is largely because the white man, except in a few favored places, suffers from tropical diseases more than does the native, and his children tend to move away if strong, or to be weaklings who die young and leave few children. It is notorious that India contains almost no fourth generation of Indian-born British. The British children are either sent back to Europe to recover their health, or else become enfeebled and their descendants die out. Even with the help of modern medical science, it is not yet certain that the permanent white population can increase greatly, although sojourners are sure to become numerous. In Australia, to be sure, the white man seems to be succeeding within the tropics, but he is still new there and has the inestimable advantage of active natural selection and freedom from contact with the natives. In many well-populated tropical countries modern science lowers the death rate among the natives, and thus increases their numbers. The white man has permitted the native popula- tion of India to double and that of Java to increase seven-fold, partly by conquering diseases and partly by the prevention of famine and war. If the conclusion just reached is correct, it THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 59 seems probable that tropical countries will long continue to maintain a dull, unprogressive population. Contact with such a population constantly exposes the white man to a most deterio- rating influence. For example, the inferior mental ability of the lower race, and its incapacity for effective organization lead to the abuse of its labor and to its exploitation in some form of peonage, even though the fact may be disguised by legal phrase- ology. Again, the presence of a despised race is almost certain to lead to low sexual morality. In the same way, political equal- ity becomes a mere form of speech, for the dominant race will not permit the other to gain rights at its expense. Manual labor, too, is despised, for it is associated with the idea of an inferior race. All these things may be looked upon as disadvantages of the lower race, but I believe that the higher reaps by far the greater injury. The conditions just mentioned appear to be among the most potent factors in rendering it difficult for the white man to attain as much success in tropical regions as in those farther to the north or south. Their evil effect is roughly proportional to the difference between the two races. That dif- ference is at a maximum where a low tropical race remains in its original, unstimulating environment, and is brought into contact with immigrants of a highly developed race who com- pletely change their environment. The newcomers are released from old restraints at a time when they stand in peculiar need of them. Instead of being stimulated to greater love of political freedom and equality, sterner morality, and more intense in- dustry, as was the case among the settlers in New England, the immigrants are in danger of being weakened in all of these re- spects. The effect on the original immigrants is bad enough, but on their children it is far worse. The settler, or European colonist, who is possessed of wealth and power, can to a slight degree shield his family, but even in such cases the children are '"n constant contact with servants. They grow up with a supreme contempt for the natives, and at the same time with the feeling 60 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE that they can treat them as they choose. If poorer people, that is, colonists in the ordinary sense of the word, attempt to live in the tropics, especially if they are people who work with their hands, their children are exposed still more to all the con- taminating influences of contact with the natives. Hence, the second and third generations, and the fourth and fifth, if there are any, suffer more than their ancestors. The degree to which the indirect or external handicaps of tropical countries are effective in lowering the standards of civilization depends largely upon the amount of energy and will power possessed by the inhabitants. This, in turn, depends upon physiological conditions. Obviously, diseases have much to do with the matter. This subject has been so much discussed that I shall here refer to it only briefly. There can be little doubt that malaria and the many other diseases which are characteristic of tropical countries play an important part in causing a low state of civilization. The old idea that the people who live in tropical regions are immune to local diseases is no longer accepted by students of tropical medicine. Adults, to be sure, are often immune, but apparently this is only partially true of children. Vast numbers of children die in infancy and early childhood from the diseases which prevent the white man from permanently living within the tropics. Others suffer, but recover. They bear the results with them to the grave, however, in the form of enlarged spleens, or other injuries to the internal organs of the body. The world has of late been astonished at the ravages of pellagra and of other diseases due to such organisms as the hookworm. People who are subject to them cannot be highly competent. Their mental processes, as well as their physical activity, are dulled. So long as a community is constantly afflicted with such disorders, it can scarcely rise high in the scale of civilization. Nothing is more hopeful for the tropics than the rapid progress in the control of these diseases. If they could be eliminated, not only might the white man be able to THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 61 live permanently where now he can be only a sojourner, but the native races would probably be greatly benefited. How great this benefit would be we cannot yet tell, but the elimination of the diseases which especially affect children would probably do much to increase vitality, energy, and initiative. This in itself would be an immeasurable boon not only to the natives, but to the white man, who would thereby be freed in part from some of his worst social dangers. This highly desirable result cannot be obtained quickly. The achievements of the United States in Panama are sometimes said to prove that diseases can be eliminated anywhere in tropical countries. This is true, but it must be remembered that Panama is a highly specialized case. During the building of the Canal a great number of people were collected in a small area, and enormous sums of money were freely expended. Everyone was subject to strict, semi-military rule, and similar conditions still continue. Such methods cannot be applied to millions of square miles. The expense would be prohibitive. The ordinary farmer in tropical regions cannot expect to be protected by his govern- ment. He must protect himself. In the long run even tropical races may learn this lesson, but it will be a difficult and expensive task, and will require a radical change in the people themselves. Such a change will doubtless come, but not for generations, and not until a long selective process has gone on whereby those who do not adopt modern medical methods will gradually be eliminated, while those who adopt them will persist. There has been so much misunderstanding of Panama and so many wild statements that it may be well to set forth the exact facts. The Health Department of the Panama Canal, as it is now called, has charge of three districts, whose population in 1917 was as follows: the city of Panama, 61,074; the city of Colon, 25,386 ; and the Canal Zone, 27,543. For purposes of health and sanitation all are under the control of the United States, and no expense is spared to make them as healthful as possible. In order 62 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE to avoid the complications due to the influenza epidemic of 1918, let us take the period from 1912 to 1917. By 1912 the health measures of the United States Army had reached such perfection that the death rate had been reduced 50 per cent. The improve- ment still continues, but it is now slow and apparently does little more than keep pace with the similar improvement in the ad- vanced parts of the world. The two cities of Panama and Colon contain the ordinary mixed population of tropical seaports : negroes from the West Indies ; Mestizos half Spanish, half Indian from the neighboring parts of Central and South America ; a few Chinese and other Asiatics ; some Europeans and Americans. A considerable number of the employees of the Canal live there. The Canal Zone, on the other hand, contains a large proportion of Canal employees, chiefly Americans, West Indian negroes, and Europeans. Among all of these the percentage of men between twenty and fifty years of age is large. The follow- ing figures show the crude death rates from 1912 to 1917 among the civilian population, excluding soldiers, in the three districts of Panama and in certain other areas with which comparisons may profitably be made. Panama . 30.5 Chile . . . 27.9 Bombay (1910-1912) . . 37.0 Colon . . 24.8 Spain . . . 22.0 Calcutta (1910-1912) . . 26.1 Canal Zone. 13.6 United States . 13.9 Amsterdam (1901-1913) . 12.6 A multitude of other figures might be presented all of which would show that while the work done in Panama has been ad- mirable, the general conditions of health in the cities of Panama and Colon are still twice as bad as in the advanced parts of the world. They are about on a par with those of similar cities of India, for Bombay and Calcutta, by reason of their size and desperate overcrowding, presumably have higher death rates than do Indian cities as small as Panama and Colon. The death rate for infants under one year bears out this general conclusion, as appears from the following figures showing the deaths per one thousand births in 1915, 1916, and 1917: THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 63 Panama, Colon, and the Canal Zone 232 Colored people in New York 182 Colored people in United States Registration Area .... 172 White people in United States Registration Area .... 96 White people in Minnesota 69 The area where births are registered in the United States in- cludes only a small part of the South, so that the death rate among colored infants as a whole is higher than appears above. In Richmond, Virginia, during 1917, 1918, and 1919, it aver- aged 198. In cities farther south it doubtless reaches a level as high as that for people of all sorts at Panama. The foregoing data make it obvious that the widespread idea as to the healthfullness of Panama is based solely on the small number of people in the Canal Zone. But the death rate of 13.6 given above for the Canal Zone has by no means the significance that is usually supposed. Its use for comparative purposes is vitiated by two facts ; first, the number of deaths by violence, chiefly by accident, is unusually high in the Canal Zone ; and second, the inhabitants of the Canal Zone are a highly selected group mostly of good physique and in the prime of life and hence bound to have a relatively low death rate no matter where they live. The best way to make a fair comparison is to take people of the same age, sex, and occupation who have otherwise also been selected by the same method, and compare the death rates in different places. But this is impossible. As the next best thing let us take the death rate from 1912 to 1917 among the Canal's white employees from the United States and compare it with the rate for men of similar age elsewhere. If we assume that the pro- portion of white men of different ages in the Canal Zone is the same as among the white employees from the United States and was also the same at the census of 1920 as in the period from 1912 to 1917, both of which are essentially the case, it is easy to compute the relative death rate in other regions on the same basis as the rate for Panama. Using the data prepared by the International Institute of Statistics together with the records 64 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE of Yale University, we find that if the proportion of men of vari- ous ages were the same in the other places as among the white American employees of the Panama Canal, the death rates among such men would be as follows : Approximate death rate when deaths Death rate from due to violence all causes are eliminated New York State, 1906-1915 12.3 10.6 Connecticut, 1906-1915 10.8 9.3 Washington State, 1908-1913 7.8 5.3 New Zealand, 1906-1915 6.7 4.8 White Canal employees from the United States, 1912-1917 4.9 2.7 Students in Yale University, 1912-1917 . . 1.9 1.7 Does this mean that the climate of the states of New York and Connecticut is relatively bad, while that of Panama and the home of Yale University at New Haven is remarkably good? Not at all. It simply means that the two states have relatively normal death rates for their particular climates and for a comparatively unselected population. They are handicapped by their numer- ous unhealthful factories and cities and by the great number of their immigrants, many of whom are poor, ignorant, and of low caliber mentally. Moreover, many of the more energetic young people have migrated westward. The adventurous and persistent qualities which lead to migration are partly due to health and physical vigor, and partly to mental initiative, adaptability, and readiness to try a new life and new methods. It needs no demon- stration to show that such people are sure to have a low death rate, especially when they are highly prosperous, as in the state of Washington. They have the physical vigor to withstand dis- ease, they have the good sense to take care of themselves, and they have the means wherewith to purchase good food, good shelter, good sanitation, and good medical service. Since New Zealand is harder to reach than Washington, its immigrants THE WHITE MAN IN TPIE TROPICS 65 have been even more highly selected for thrift, health, and physical and mental vigor. Panama, like Washington and New Zealand, attracts chiefly the more vigorous type of people. The man who is organically diseased rarely thinks of going there. Moreover, in Panama white employees come from America as adults. On the contrary, many of the people of Washington and New Zealand were born there and have remained regardless of whether they possess the pioneer vigor and initiative of their parents. Again, even if they have the brave spirit that overcomes physical handicaps, the or- ganically weak are not allowed to go to Panama as employees. If they try to go, they are weeded out by physical examinations. Even that, however, does not end the matter, for the examina- tions are repeated each year. Every individual who shows signs of weakness is advised to leave Panama as soon as possible; many are ordered home ; and not a few are deported, especially those suffering from mental disorders. During the three years for which I have been able to find data (1914, 1915, and 1917) the deportations on account of disease among employees of all sorts, both white and colored, amounted to approximately 40 per cent of the total deaths from disease. If these people had stayed in Panama, as they stay in New York, Connecticut, Washington, or New Zealand, many of them would soon have died. Inasmuch as such deportations have been going on for years, it is practically certain that without them the death rate at Panama would be decidedly larger than now. In addition to the persons who are deported, a far larger number go home voluntarily on the advice of their physicians. Moreover, many who show no immediate signs of disease remain at home after one of their earlier furloughs because they find the climate at Panama uncomfortable. In addition to all this it must be remembered that the white employees at Panama are practically all officials or clerks ; they belong to a class of society which by reason of its intelligence is 66 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE able to take care of itself, so that its death rate is normally much lower than that of the great body of men of similar age. More- over, the employees are well paid and admirably housed. They likewise have long and frequent vacations at home whereby the effect of the tropical climate is partially neutralized. All these conditions, even without the excellent medical care which the employees receive, free of cost, would insure a degree of health in Panama much better than in most tropical regions. On the other hand, if the population of Panama were an ordinary un- selected type and if none of the weak or sick were sent away, it seems probable that in spite of admirable sanitation and medical care the death rate would be larger than in New York among people of similar age. This last statement is merely an opinion, since by its very nature it is not susceptible of actual proof. We know as a fact, however, that the death rate at Panama is greatly lowered by the selection of healthy, intelligent employees as well as by good medical care. The conditions at Yale or any other university suggest that in such cases selection is even more important than medical care. From June, 1912, to June, 1917, the average number of under- graduates at Yale was 2476, among whom the total number of deaths was nine through disease and one by accident. This gives an annual death rate of 0.8 per thousand against 4.5 for all the young men of Connecticut between 15 and 24 years of age. In other words, an unselected young man in Connecticut is 5.6 times as likely to die as is a selected Yale student. If a similar ratio prevailed among the University men up to the age of about 55 years, and if the proportion of men at each age were the same as at Panama, their death rate, barring accidents, would be only 1.7 against 2.7 among the white American employees at Panama. Now as a matter of fact, from the standpoint of health the employees at Panama are far more rigidly selected than are the Yale students. No medical examination is required for entrance to the University, no one is actually sent away because of his THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 67 health, and the amount of medical attention is less on the whole than at Panama. Other things being equal, this ought to give a higher death rate at Yale than at Panama. Yet, as a matter of fact, it actually gives Panama the higher rate by 60 per cent. This turning of the tables against Panama seems to be due to the adverse climate. The net result of the preceding investigation is this : There can be no doubt of the great value and success of the medical and sanitary work at Panama. It has cut the death rate in halves at the cities of Panama and Colon. Nevertheless, the death rate in those cities is still extremely high, about twice that of the United States as a whole. So far as the white people at Panama are con- cerned, the death rate is very low, but that proves nothing about the climate. It merely proves that it is possible to obtain prac- tically any death rate by selecting the cases. One could go to hospitals and select the critical cases. That might give a death rate of eight or nine hundred. One might select college athletes and from time to time throw out any who showed signs of illness, and the death rate would be zero. But to use such death rates as evidence concerning the climate would be highly misleading. It is poor policy to use any such reasoning in respect to Panama, Northern Australia, or any other region where the climate pos- sesses disadvantages. To do so encourages false hopes. When these are disappointed, people tend to blame the whole science of tropical medicine. That science is doing wonderful things, but as yet there is no evidence that it has overcome the effects of climate, although it has certainly mitigated them. We shall re- turn to this subject in connection with Australia. There, as in Panama, the tropical death rate is lower than those of better climates, but this is due primarily to the selection of certain types of residents. I have dwelt on this matter because there is a vast deal of misapprehension and very little realization of the importance of selection. Let us return now to our main question. Suppose that the 68 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE white man should succeed in cultivating the tropical forests, transversing the waste places, and conquering the diseases. Sup- pose also that he should eliminate the deteriorating influences of low social and moral standards among the natives. But suppose also that there were no selection of the white colonists. If all this were suddenly done, and average unselected white men were set down in a tropical garden of Eden, would they be able to hold their own among the peoples of the world? Would Teutons or Latins under such circumstances be able permanently to main- tain as high a standard of civilization as is maintained by their brothers in Europe? Or would there be a change in some of the traits which we are wont to call racial? Clearly we are back at the point where we started, and are confronted by the question of race versus place. We must determine how much of our Euro- pean and American energy, initiative, persistence, and other qualities upon which we so much pride ourselves is due to racial inheritance, and how much to residence under highly stimulating conditions of climate. One of the lines along which we may seek for an answer is by a comparison of the character of Europeans in tropical coun- tries with their character in the temperate zone. Whatever differ- ences we may find are presumably due partly to physiological and partly to sociological causes, but they manifest themselves chiefly through the will. In tropical countries weakness of will is unfortunately displayed not only by the natives, but by a large proportion of the northerner sojourners. It manifests itself in many ways. Four of these, namely, lack of industry, an irascible temper, drunkenness, and sexual indulgence are par- ticularly prominent, and may be taken as typical. Others, such as proneness to gambling and disregard for the truth, might equally well be considered if space allowed. In the quality of industry the difference between people in tropical and other countries is well known. We have already touched on it in the Bahamas, but let us amplify it further. THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 69 Practically every northerner who goes to the torrid zone says at first that he works as well as at home, and that he finds the climate delightful. He may even be stimulated to unusual exertion. Little by little, however, even though he retains perfect health, he slows down. He does not work so hard as before, nor does the spirit of ambition prick him so keenly. On the low, damp seacoast, and still more in the lowland forests, the process of deterioration is relatively rapid, although its duration may vary enormously in different individuals. In the dry interior the process is slower, and on the high plateaus it may take many years. Both in books and in conversation with inhabitants of tropical regions one finds practical unanimity as to this tropical inertia, and it applies both to body and mind. After long sojourn in the tropics it is hard to spur one's self to the physical effort of a mountain climb, and equally hard to think out the steps in a long chain of reasoning. The mind, like the body, wants rest. Both can be spurred to activity, but this exhausts vitality. The common ex- planations of tropical inertia are diverse. One man says that within the tropics hard work is unnecessary, because salaries are high ; another asserts that it is because servants are cheap ; still another claims that hard work is dangerous to the health ; and almost all agree that "anyhow, one doesn't feel like working down here." Probably all four of these factors cooperate, and each, doubtless, produces pronounced results, but the last two, health and "feeling," seem the most important. In spite of individual exceptions, white men who spur them- selves to exert their minds as earnestly and steadily within the tropics as at home are in great danger of breaking down in health. They become nervous and enfeebled, and readily suc- cumb to tropical diseases. This is one of the most powerful deterrents to the development of an efficient white population in equatorial regions. If the more intellectual members of the community ruin their health, they are almost sure to die before their time, or else to go back to the North. In either case they 70 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE are not likely to leave many children to perpetuate their char- acteristics. Thus if white colonization takes place on a large scale within the tropics, there is grave danger that the physically strong but mentally lethargic elements will be the ones to be- come the ancestors of the future population. In the past this factor must have operated to weed out the more intellectual members of each of the many races that have migrated toward the equator. The inertia which prevents the less competent mem- bers of a tropical community from overworking may perhaps be interpreted by teleologists as a merciful provision of Provi- dence to warn man that he must not work too hard in the torrid zone, but that will scarcely help to advance civilization. Few people will question the reality of the tropical inertia. It is the same lassitude which everyone feels on a hot summer day the inclination to sit down and dream, the tendency to hesitate before beginning a piece of work, and to refrain from plunging into the midst of it in the energetic way which seems natural under more stimulating conditions. Lack of will power is shown by northerners in tropical regions not only in loss of energy and ambition, but in fits of anger. The English official who returns from India is commonly described as "choleric." Every traveler in tropical countries knows that he sometimes bursts into anger in a way that makes him utterly ashamed, and which he would scarcely believe possible at home. Almost any American or European who has traveled or resided within the tropics will confess that he has occasionally flown into a passion, and perhaps used physical violence, under cir- cumstances which at home would merely have made him vexed. This is due apparently to four chief causes. One is the ordinary tropical diseases, for when a man has a touch of fever, his temper is apt to get the better of him. In the second place, the slowness of tropical people is terribly exasperating. The impatient north- erner uses every possible means to make the natives hurry, or to compel them to keep their word. His energy is usually wasted THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 71 the native remains unmoved, and the only visible result is an angry and ridiculous foreigner. Yet a show of anger and violence often seems to be the only way of getting things done, and this is frequently used as an excuse for lack of self-control. In the third place, the consequences of becoming angry arc less dan- gerous than elsewhere. The inert people of tropical countries often submit to indignities which an ordinary white man would bitterly resent. Of course they object to ill treatment, and will retaliate if possible, but they generally do not have sufficient energy or cunning to make their vengeance effective against the powerful white man. Finally, those who have lived in the tropics generally find that, even when things go smoothly, and they are in contact with people of their own kind and are in compara- tively good health, they are more irritable than at home. In other words, their power of self-control is enfeebled. Of course there are many exceptions, but that does not affect the genera] principle. Drunkenness, our third evidence of lack of self-control, need scarcely be discussed. Within the tropics, the white man's alco- hol in the form of rum is scarcely more injurious to the natives of Africa than it is in other forms to himself. In places such as Guatemala and parts of Mexico, drunken men and women may be seen upon the streets at almost any time of day. Nowhere else, during extensive travels in America, Europe, and Asia, have I seen so much drunkenness as in Guatemala. Among white men a large number drink as badly as the natives. Here is an example ; a railway conductor was telling me about drinks in Guatemala: "They've got something here called 'white-eye,' " he remarked. "You know that Mexican 'mescal,' and how strong it is? Well, white-eye's got mescal chained to a telegraph pole. Yes, I drink it. A man's got to drink something. The first time I tried it, I got crazy drunk and smashed things up the way they all do. I was arrested and fined fifty dollars. [This is really only two and a half, for Guatemalan currency consists of non-redeemable 72 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE paper, which at that time was worth about five cents on a dollar a characteristic evidence of tropical incapacity.] I got fined several times that way and didn't like it. Then one day when I was going to get drunk, I said to myself, Til go and pay my fine now and then they won't bother me.' I did that several times, and the *jefe politico' liked it [presumably because it was an easy way of pocketing the money]. Then he said to the police : 'Don't bother this man. Just let him get drunk all he likes, and he'll pay his fines at the proper time.' I tell you, white-eye is bad stuff. The only proper way to drink it is to take a quart bottle in the morning. Find a place that will stay shady all day. Drink the whole thing right down and get so dead drunk that you will sleep till night." I do not cite this man as typical of all the white men in the tropics. Far from it. Many conduct themselves with sobriety and industry, but such men almost invariably make frequent and protracted visits to the better climate of the North. If a white man stays steadily for long periods in the tropics, however, and if his character has any weak spots, they are almost sure to be exaggerated. The drunkenness of the tropical white man arises in part from the constant heat, which makes people want some- thing to drink at all times, partly from the monotony of life, and still more from the absence of the social restraints which exercise so powerful an inhibitory influence at home. Back of all these things, however, among both the white men and natives, there seem to lie two conditions which arc directly connected with the climate. One is the same cnfecblement of the will which makes a man burst into anger. The other is a constant feeling of inefficiency which makes a man crave something to brace him up. The last of the ways in which weakness of will is evident in tropical countries is the relation of the sexes. Its importance can scarcely be overestimated. It leads to the ruin of thousands of northerners, even though they do not yield to drink, to anger, THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 73 or to laziness. When once they have fallen into pronounced im- morality the other weaknesses soon follow. The condition of the native races is still worse. Everywhere within the tropics mis- sionaries say that their converts can be taught honesty, in- dustry, and many other virtues, but that even the strongest find it almost impossible to resist the temptations of sex. Many Europeans condone this. They say that it is natural, and that the natives had better be left to their own conventional ways of restricting but not preventing sexual intercourse. Perhaps they are right, although I cannot be certain. That is not the point, however. We are at present concerned with the effect which free indulgence has upon civilization and upon the capacity for progress. This may be illustrated by what Gouldsbury and Sheane, for example, say of the Zulus in northern Rhodesia. They hold that one of the greatest reasons why these people remain so backward is that their thought and energy are largely swallowed up in matters of sex. During the years when the young men ought to be getting new ideas and thinking out the many little projects and the few great ones which combine to cause progress, the vast majority are thinking of women, and planning to gain possession of some new woman or girl. Under such circumstances no race can rise to any high position. The causes of these conditions are various. Many writers dis- miss the matter by saying that the social standards of tropical people are low and tend to cause northerners to conform to them. This is true, but it explains nothing. A real, though minor, rea- son for the lowness of the standards is found in the free, open life which is almost universal within the tropics. People are out of doors so much and it is so easy to meet in secret that tempta- tion arises very frequently. Much more important is the scanty dress of the women, and its character, which calls attention to their sex. Livingstone speaks with disgust of the way in which his carriers, hour after hour, discussed the breasts of the half- naked women whom they met. Even in the North women seem to 74 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE be strangely indifferent to the effect of their mode of dress upon men. They do not seem to think that they are responsible if their low-necked gowns and the making of their clothing in such a way that each little movement of their bodies can be detected, stir men's passions. They appear oblivious to the fact that the display of their beauty often means that some other woman must pay the penalty. Within the tropics these conditions are exag- gerated. I believe I am speaking within bounds when I say that any young man of European race with red blood in his veins is in more danger of deteriorating in character and efficiency be- cause of the women of the tropics than from any other single cause. The strength of this deteriorating force is not merely exter- nal. Either the actual temptation to sexual excess is greater in the tropics than elsewhere, or else the inhibitory forces are weakened by the same processes which cause people to drink to excess, to become unduly angry, and to work slowly. Hcllpach states that it is said that in southern Italy sexual irregularities increase greatly at times when the hot, damp wind known as the sirocco blows across the Mediterranean from the deserts of northern Africa. This is so well recognized among the people themselves that offenses committed under such circumstances are in a measure condoned. Violence, too, is more common at such times, for self-control of every kind is weakened. In eastern Turkey the hot desert winds cause the whole community to be- come cross and irritable. I have there seen a missionary, a man of unusual strength of character, shut himself up in his study all day, because he knew that he was in danger of saying something disagreeable. I cite this case because, among the people whom I have known, missionaries are, on the whole, most completely masters of themselves and the least likely to let minor circum- stances turn them from the Christlike lives which they are striv- ing to live day by day before the native communities. For this same reason, to return to our immediate subject, I quote the THE WHITE MAN IN THE TROPICS 75 remark of a missionary in Central America when we were dis- cussing the morality of the country. He was a most austere man, a member of a small and extremely devout sect, and his whole being was devoted to preaching the gospel. Speaking of his own experiences, he said: "When I am in this country, evil spirits seem to attack me. I suppose you would call them something else, but that is what I think they are. When I am at home in the United States I feel pure and true, but when I come here, it seems as if lust were written in the very faces of the people." In all the evils which have just been mentioned laziness, anger, drunkenness, and immorality social causes undoubtedly play an important part. A strong public opinion would save many a young northerner from drink and immorality, and would keep him faithful to his work. A clear religious faith or a high ideal of duty would do the same thing. Good homes, proper dress, and many other material changes would help greatly. So, too, would a study of how it has come to pass that certain tropical races, in spite of their environment, have developed compara- tively high moral codes to which they strictly adhere, while a few have actually learned the lesson of industry. Along with the social aspect of the question, however, and neither more nor less important, goes the physical. We must discover to exactly what extent physical conditions help or hinder the development of strong character. That is the purpose of the chapters that follow. CHAFTEK IV THE EFFECT OF THE SEASONS IN comparing Teutons with negroes, or tropical people with those of the temperate zone, we have been following a method as old as the days of Aristotle. Such comparisons have led to most interesting generalizations not only at the hands of Aris- totle himself, but of many other men such as Montesquieu, Hurn- boldt, and Ratzel. Yet the importance of climate as a factor in civilization is still in doubt. For instance, no one denies that South Africa is confronted by a grave race problem, but many say that it is purely economic, and has nothing to do with cli- mate. They support this view by strong arguments. Thus we are left in uncertainty. The only way to remove this is to devise some method whereby to separate the effects of climate from those due to all other causes, whether economic, historic, social, religious, racial, or something else. Accordingly, the rest of this volume will be devoted to an investigation of the exact effect of various climatic factors upon selected groups of people, and to an attempt to discover how human energy and other qualities would be distributed if all the earth's inhabitants were influenced like these particular groups. In the study of climate one of the most puzzling features is the diversity of opinion among persons of good judgment. For instance, at what season do people work fastest in the northern United States? Some will say the winter, some the spring, a considerable number the fall, and a few the summer. Most will say that they are least efficient in summer, but others believe that they are at their worst in the early spring or late winter. EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 77 Again, ask a dozen friends whether they work best on clear days or cloudy. The majority will probably answer that the first clear day after a storm is by all means the best. A small number will perhaps think the matter over more carefully, and then say that after a storm the clearness of the air and the brightness of the sun are certainly inspiring, but one really accomplishes more when it rains. This divergence of opinion is due largely to the fact that climatic effects are of two kinds, psychological and physiologi- cal. We are always conscious of the first, but often unconscious of the second. The two are admirably distinguished in Hellpach's book on Geopsychische Erscheinungen. An example will make the matter clear. It is well known that at high altitudes the num- ber of red corpuscles in the blood increases enormously, and the capacity to absorb oxygen and to give out carbon dioxide is correspondingly modified. Yet many people can go to altitudes of 5000 feet or more without realizing that their physiological functions have been altered. To cite my own case, up to the age of twenty-one I had never been a thousand feet above the sea. Then I went to live at an altitude of 4500 feet. The only physio- logical effect of which I was conscious was unusual sleepiness for the first few months, but whether this was due to the altitude or to the dryness of the air, I do not know. For two or three years I never thought of the physiological effect of the altitude until one day, happening to have climbed to a height of 7000 feet, I began to run up hill. I lost my breath and became tired so quickly that I was alarmed and thought I must be sick. I was much re- lieved when it occurred to me that the altitude was not favorable for running up hill. Manifestly my physiological functions were different from what they were at sea level, although I was un- conscious of it. On the other hand, psychologically I was daily conscious of living in a place where the air was extraordinarily clear, and where the mountains were always in sight across a splendid plain twelve hundred feet below us. Presumably both 78 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the physiological and psychological conditions had an appre- ciable effect upon the work of every day, but which was the greater it is impossible to tell. In this connection Lchmann and Pcdersen state an interesting fact. In Denmark and Norway they made a series of daily tests of the strength of three individuals by means of a dynamometer. They found that the change of atmospheric pressure due to an ascent of two or three thousand feet makes no appreciable dif- ference. A similar descent, however, is accompanied by a marked increase of strength which disappears within three or four days. They suggest that this may be due to the persistence of abun- dant red corpuscles when people come down from high places. The red corpuscles multiply very rapidly under the influence of low pressure, but are slower in disappearing when the pres- sure once more increases. Thus, for the first day or two after a person has come down from the mountains, more than the normal amount of oxygen may be absorbed, and muscular strength correspondingly increased. Possibly this is why mountaineers are generally so irresistible when they descend upon the plains in sudden raids. My colleague, Prof. H. E. Gregory, suggests that this may account for the fact that in the horse-races of the pioneer days of the southwestern United States, the poor, scrawny animals brought down from the mountains by the In- dians usually belied their appearance and outran the better- looking animals of the white men. They may have had an excess of red corpuscles. Professor Gregory adds that in some of the highland regions of South America there is a strict rule that before a race the competing horses must spend a certain number of days at the race course. This may have arisen because the animals which race directly after coming from the mountains are apt to win. There is considerable doubt as to the truth of this theory, but it illustrates the possibility that we may be deeply influenced by atmospheric conditions of which we are almost unconscious. EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 79 In our opinions as to the effect of the seasons or of daily changes of weather the relation between psychological and physiological influences is probably the same as in the case of altitude. The external conditions which we see and feel make a greater impression than those which prevail within our bodies. For example, most of us think that in the northern United States we work fast in winter. As a matter of fact, the statistics of ten thousand people show that we work slowly. The ordinary im- pression is apparently psychological. In order to keep warm out of doors in winter we walk fast and this leads us to think that we do everything rapidly. Again, the blue sky, clear air, bright sunshine, and fresh colors of the first day after a storm are unquestionably inspir- ing, but does that inspiration make us work any better? May it not lead to a nervous excitement which actually hinders our work, by causing us to look out at the beauties of nature or to be less concentrated in other ways? The actual figures show that, taking the year as a whole, on dull days, especially the second such day when a storm begins to clear, we accomplish more than on bright days, even though we grumble about the clouds and the dampness. A bright day certainly makes us cheerful, but its chief helpfulness, so far as our work is concerned, is felt when it is a change from the monotony of a series of dull days. Clouds and rain produce exactly as much rejoicing when they succeed prolonged clear weather of the kind that we praise so highly. In America I have never seen so much rejoicing over a bright day as I have seen in Turkey when the first rain fell after the long subtropical summer with its truly superb weather. The rejoic- ing was in part due to the fact that the coming of the rains means good crops, but I have again and again seen exuberant joy among people to whom the crops made no difference whatever. I have seen Americans shout for joy because the clouds had come, and run out into the rain to let the cool drops refresh their faces. 80 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE The questions which have just been asked and the possibili- ties that have been suggested show how indefinite are our ideas of the effect of climate. We understand its psychological effects fairly well. We know little of its physiological effects, however, except when they are extreme or unusual, or when people are sick or are in some other pathological condition. We need to determine how ordinary people are influenced by ordinary con- ditions of weather. That is the purpose of our present discus- sion. The most feasible way to do this, as has already been said, is to take groups of people who live in a variable climate, and measure their efficiency under different conditions of weather. The best and fullest test of efficiency is a person's daily work. If the subject does not know that he is being tested, so much the better. Piece-workers in factories are doing exactly what is required for our purpose. Accordingly, to begin with New Eng- land, I have taken the daily records of about 300 men and 250 girls, most of them for a complete year. The records are dis- tributed over the four years from 1910 to 1913. The 550 people were emplo^yed in three factories in the cities of Bridgeport, New Britain, and New Haven, in the southwestern part of Con- necticut. In all cases the officials in charge of the factories were most courteous and helpful in assisting me to obtain the neces- sary data, and I wish most warmly to express my gratitude to all concerned. In the selection of operatives for such a purpose, various con- ditions must be fulfilled. In the first place, they must be piece- workers who are paid according to their work and not at a fixed rate per day. In the second place, they must be employed in factories where their output is not limited by restrictions im- posed by unions, or by the fear that if they earn too much, wages will be reduced. They must be doing work that is of essentially the same kind every day, so that their wages will not vary much because thev are sometimes engaged upon new and unfamiliar tasks, or upon easy tasks at some times and hard ones at others. EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 81 Furthermore, the same people must work steadily for month after month, throughout the year, if possible, and without tak- ing much time off, as is such a common practice among factory hands. Finally, they must be working where there is abundant incentive to steady, faithful work, where the conditions of air and light are reasonably good, and where accurate daily records make it possible to determine not only the daily wages of each individual but the average efficiency per hour or per day of standard length. The number of factories where all these condi- tions are fulfilled is small, for they demand special types of occupation and a high standard of management. The three factories from which data have been obtained all meet the re- quirements. I explained what I wanted to the superintendent or to some other responsible official in each case. He then selected the group or groups of operatives whom he thought proper, and placed the figures in my hands. There was no selection on my part, and in each case I have used all the figures, omitting only a few obvious errors amounting to perhaps a quarter of one per cent. An investigation such as is here set forth may follow two modes of procedure. One is to take a few persons and investigate each minutely in order to eliminate all accidental variations. The other is to take many people and get rid of the personal variations by averages. The wages of a workman depend upon many factors aside from the weather. One man has been scolded by his wife because he did not earn enough last week, another wants to buy some clothes for his little boy, and a third was drunk last night. A sore toe may have far more influence than any possible climatic variation. To ferret out all these accidental circumstances is out of the question. Fortunately, they do not occur every day, and most people work weeks at a time without being much influenced by them. Moreover, when large numbers of people work in different cities and during different years, the individual circumstances neutralize one another. The dav that 82 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE John Jenkins is disturbed because his boy has run away, Tony Albano is working hard because he is going to be married. Hence, by taking five hundred people we are able to eliminate accidental and individual circumstances and thus to reach a reliable result. All three of the factories whence our data are obtained make hardware, but the work varies greatly. In one factory where Italians are the predominant nationality, brass sockets for elec- tric lights, and other little brass fittings are made. One group of people was here engaged in tending machines. Some were turn- ing out screws, others were putting pieces of sheet brass into automatic machines which turn out perforated plates. The work requires little skill, but much quickness and concentration. An- other group, composed largely of Italians, was engaged in roll- ing and drawing hot brass, a heavy and somewhat difficult kind of work, requiring considerable strength. It is difficult because the brass must be used hot, and hence the men must work in abnormally high temperatures. At another factory, the one from which the largest number of records was obtained during three successive years, there were two main groups of men and two of women. The girls, from sixteen to twenty years of age, were Americans by birth, but of varied descent, being chiefly Irish, Germans, Scandinavians, English, and other north Euro- peans. Their work was the packing of hinges and screws, which are first wrapped in tissue paper and then placed in pasteboard boxes. This is a light, easy task in which dexterity and accuracy in picking up the right number of pieces are particularly im- portant. For the first week or two when screws are packed, the tips of the fingers become sore, which makes the work proceed slowly. If a girl is changed from packing hinges to packing screws, her wages fall off for a time, but such changes are not frequent, and do not appreciably influence our figures. The men at this factory were of all ages, and were of the same races as the girls. They were engaged in grinding and buffing the hinges. The first operation is hard, heavy work. The hinges are held EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 83 upon rapidly revolving emery wheels in order to grind them to a smooth surface. The other operation, buffing, is similar except that it is easier, for the hinges after being ground are polished upon rapidly revolving cloth buffs covered with emery dust. In the third factory, the operatives were of north European de- scent, almost all being native-born. Practically all, both girls and boys, were young, only a few being much over twenty years of age. The older girls leave to be married, and the boys, who are comparatively few in number, go elsewhere to find harder and hence better paid work. The work consists of the preparation of armatures and other wire coils for electrical purposes. Some operatives wind the wire upon rapidly revolving spools. Others put together the various parts of an armature. The work is light and not tiresome. It requires much dexterity and accuracy. Strings have to be tied at particular spots, pieces of paper must be inserted, the machines must be stopped when the right point has been reached, and little ends have to be grasped and inserted in their proper places. Taking our three factories together, the work ranges from the hardest to the lightest. It is of many kinds, requiring different degrees of strength and skill. The wages depend not only upon the amount of work completed, but upon the number of pieces rejected. In other words, the wages represent not only speed, but accuracy. Let us now turn to the actual performance of the operatives. This is summed up in Figure 1. The four upper solid lines repre- sent the work done week after week, each year from 1910 to 1913. In Figure 1 the work of only about 410 people has been used. The rest have been omitted because the figures are not complete for a whole year. In only one case has there been a deliberate omission of figures which cover an entire year. That was the Italians who draw hot brass and hence are subject to abnormal conditions of temperature. The method of procedure has been to find for each working day the average hourly wages for each group of operatives. Hourly wages have been used in- Jan Feh Mck Apr May Jirte. Jify Avy&pt Oet Nw.D**. zo Figure 1. 'I'hc Effect of the Seasons on Factory Operatives in Connecticut (solid lines) and at Pittsburgh (dotted lines) EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 85 stead of daity, so as to make it possible to compare half-days with whole. If part of the operatives were absent on any par- ticular day, they were simply omitted, and the average for the rest was taken. When the daily averages had been found, they were averaged together by weeks. In doing this, a half-day, such as the Saturdays in summer, was given only half as much weight as a whole day, and days when part of the operatives were absent or when the machinery was shut down for a while, were given a correspondingly smaller weight. Thus allowance is everywhere made for irregularities in the number of employees and the length of time that they work. The final process consisted of combining the different groups. In order that each individual may have the same importance, all the figures have been reduced to per- centages. In this way if a girl earned a maximum wage of twelve cents an hour, it is called 100 per cent, while if a man's maxi- mum wages were thirty cents, this sum also is called 100 per cent. Thus the variations in the wages of the girl and the man have the same weight in our final computations. Because of the enor- mous amount of work which would have been entailed, it was not possible to reduce the wages of each individual to percent- ages, but only those of each group. Had it been possible to work out each individual's wages separately, the results shown in our curves would probably have been more striking than is now the case. In Figure 1 the height of the curves indicates the efficiency of the operatives at various seasons for four successive years. The fifth curve, heavier than the others, is the average of the preceding four. Turning to the upper line, we see that in early January, 1910, the efficiency of about 60 factory operatives in Bridgeport was 88 per cent as much as during the week of maximum efficiency that year. By the middle of the month it had fallen to 86 per cent. Later it rose fairly steadily to 96 per cent at the end of April. Then it dropped a little, rose still higher in June, and fell off distinctly during the summer, but 86 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE not so low as in winter. During the autumn it rose steadily until early November, when it reached the highest point of the year, after which it fell rapidly. In the same way each curve may be traced week by week. I shall return to them shortly. Meanwhile, it would be advantageous for the reader to look them over and draw his own conclusions, picking out the features which are common to all, and noting those which show different degrees of intensity from year to year. In Figure 1 it will be noticed that the solid lines never reach 100 per cent. This is partly because they have been smoothed, and partly because they have been corrected to compensate for the increased efficiency due to practice. The process of smooth- ing, as everyone knows, is used by mathematicians to eliminate minor variations and thus permit the main trend of a curve to be more apparent. It merely takes off the high points and the low. The figures for three weeks are averaged, and the average is used instead of the original figure for the middle week. In the present case, and in practically all the curves in this book, the process of smoothing has been performed twice on each curve. If the letters a to e represent the average wages for five suc- cessive weeks, the figure actually used for the middle week, c, is obtained from the following equation : a -f 2b + 3c + 2d + e 9 This process of smoothing can add nothing to a curve ; it simply takes away the less important details. If carried far enough it would produce straight lines. In addition to smoothing the curves I have corrected them for the effects of practice. The curves for 1911, and 1912, and 1913 are all based on the same factory at New Britain. When the wages for each year are averaged, we find that those for 1912 were 1.5 per cent higher than for 1911, and those for 1913 were 1.5 per cent higher than for 1912. This means that EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 87 constant practice caused the average employee, including both old hands and new, to be 1.5 per cent more skillful at the end of the year than at the beginning. Hence, from January onward, the curve rises a little until in December it is 1.5 per cent higher than it would be if the operatives had not grown more skillful. To eliminate this we simply tip the entire curve, raising the January end by three quarters of one per cent and depressing the December end by the same amount. The fluctuations, of course, remain unchanged. In Figure 1, if there had been no correction, the highest and lowest points of the upper curve would lie at the points indi- cated by the crosses, and the other curves would be changed in corresponding ratios, there being no change at the end of June. Turning to less technical matters, let us consider the degree of resemblance in the four upper solid lines of Figure 1. All are unmistakably low in January. Then from February to June we note a general rise, varied by minor fluctuations which differ from year to year. At the middle or end of June all reach a distinct maximum, although in 1912 and 1913 it is of slight proportions. Next we have a drop during the summer, pro- nounced in 1910 and 1911, but not at all prominent in 1912, and scarcely noticeable in 1913. Following this there comes a series of irregular fluctuations, differing from curve to curve, but in each case culminating in a strong maximum at the end of Octo- ber or the beginning of November. Six weeks later, in the middle of December, another slight maximum is suggested, and then all the curves drop suddenly. In the average curve the minor fluctuations tend to disappear. They are more or less accidental, and represent peculiar conditions which pertain to one year but not to others. The features that have been named, however, show no sign of disappearing. They are five in number, namely, an extremely low place in midwinter, and a less pronounced low place in midsummer ; a high point in June, a still higher point at the end of October, and a hump in mid-December. Much the most 88 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE variable feature is the low place in summer. This is highly sig- nificant, as we shall shortly see. Before we discuss the causes of the variability of the summers let us consider the meaning of the curves as a whole. In the first place, it is evident that, although details may vary from year to year, the general course of events is uniformly from low in the winter to high in the fall with a drop of more or less mag- nitude in summer. To what can this be due? Did the factories shut down in January, or run on part time, or decrease work because of lack of orders, or to overhaul the machinery and so forth? Do the high wages in October and November indicate a special rush of orders at that time? Of course, any variations in the way in which the factor}^ is running must be reflected in the wages of the operatives, but in the present case this does not apply to the main variations, although it may apply to minor details. In neither of the two factories here considered were the responsible heads able to offer any explanation of the peculi- arities of the curves on the basis of facto ry management or the exigencies of business. Both are engaged in making staple arti- cles, the chief demand for which comes in the spring when build- ing operations begin. There is no Christmas rush on hinges and electric light sockets. After Christmas the factories shut down for a few days at the beginning of the year, but that ought to increase rather than diminish the hourly earnings. When opera- tives are working only part time they feel the need of earning as much as possible each hour. If part of the hands are laid off, that would also increase the average hourly wages, for the weaker ones would be dropped, and the average ability of those who remain would be high. In this connection, it is important to understand that in these factories a man is free to work as hard as he wishes at any time of the year. The managers have deliberately adopted the policy of getting as much work as possible out of each opera- tive. Overhead charges for interest, superintendence, bookkeep- EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 89 ing, salesmen, and other outside expenses, and also the charges for unproductive labor such as engineers, janitors, and the like are no greater no matter how hard the productive employees work. If the producing operatives should double their output, most of the other expenses would scarcely increase at all. Hence, it would not only be possible to pay double wages for double work, but it would be profitable to the factory even if it paid perhaps $2.50 where now it pays $1.00. In view of these condi- tions, both factories have adopted systems whose special object is to encourage extra exertion. In one case, part of the men work upon what is known as the "premium" plan. The management and the men have agreed that the various tasks shall be rated according to the number of hours which they may fairly be sup- posed to require. If a man performs an eight-hour task, he is to be paid for eight hours' work, no matter whether he does it in six hours or ten. If, however, he finishes the work in less than the stipulated time, he goes to work at another task for the rest of the period. For half of this time he is to be paid, while the factory gets the benefit of the other half. For example, if an eight-hour task is finished in six, the operative works two more hours. He is then paid for nine hours although he has only worked eight, while the factory gets ten hours' work and pays for nine. Thus both are the gainers. In one case the managers made a mistake in deciding upon the number of hours needed for a certain task. It had never been done quickly, and no one knew how rapidly it might be done. The man who does it soon earned ten or twelve dollars a day, where he formerly earned perhaps two and a half or three. Inasmuch as the management had agreed not to change the rates, they stuck to their bargain. The task only occupies one day each month, and the matter is not serious. Moreover, even though the operative earns such high wages, the work actually costs the factory less than when he was earning two dollars and a half. In the other factory the girls are stimulated by bonuses. That 90 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE is, they are not only paid for their work, but if they do more than is expected they are paid a bonus. For example, if a girPs wages averaged about a dollar a day, and she did work worth $1.20, she did not receive $1.20, but $1.25 or even $1.40. The factory finds this worth while because so much more can be pro- duced without any increase in charges for interest, office work, and other overhead expenses. When this bonus system was first introduced, it produced only a slight effect. The girls did not seem to care about the bonuses and made little effort to get them. Then the management realized that the parents were get- ting the extra money, and so it made no difference to the girls, most of whom gave their pay envelopes unopened to their fathers or mothers. Thereafter the bonus was not put in the pay envel- ope, but was handed out in loose change. The girls kept it and began to work hard. In the third factory, whose figures are not extensive enough to be used in Figure 1, but which enter into other computations, a similar system is employed. A limit is set for each task. If the work is performed within that time a bonus is paid. Otherwise the operatives receive only the regular pay, no matter how much time they spend. The introduction of this system has increased the output of the factory enormously. Inasmuch as the various systems of bonuses and premiums are equally applicable at all times of the year, it seems impossible to find in the factories themselves any reason why earnings should be very low in January, moderately low in July, high in June, and very high in November. We seem forced to search outside of the factories for the reasons for our seasonal fluctuations of wages. Such things as panics, hard times, or strikes would certainly cause a general change in the conditions of work, but nothing of the kind oc- curred during the period under consideration. Moreover, such events do not recur at the same time each year. Aside from the seasons, the only event which recurs regularly year after year at the same time and which is important enough to cause varia- EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 91 tions in wages is Christmas. Its effect can be seen unmistakably in each of the solid year-curves. In that for 1910 it appears in the little hump which culminates during the next to the last week in December. In the other three it comes a week earlier be- cause this factory does not pay the week's wages on the Satur- day of the week in question, but a week later, after there has been time to check up the work and make allowances for that which is poorly done. Hence, money for Christmas must be earned before the middle of December. If there were no such thing as Christmas, the wages would probably drop off in the way shown by the dash line in the average curve of Figure 1. After Christmas the wages probably drop somewhat lower than would otherwise be the case, for there must be a reaction from the previous effort, but it is noticeable that the wages do not reach their lowest ebb directly after Christmas, but keep on falling for nearly a month. Something else keeps them low. The Christmas hump is significant chiefly because it shows unmis- takably that an outside stimulus which applies to all the opera- tives produces a distinct result. We may properly infer that the other permanent features of our curves are also due to some outside force which influences all the operatives. That force must be connected with the seasons, and it must be far more powerful than Christmas, for its effects are far greater. There seems to be no recourse except to ascribe the fluctuations of the curves to climate. The verity of the conclusion just reached is strongly con- firmed by comparison with other regions and other types of human activity. Figure 2, which, for convenience, is here divided into two overlapping portions, presents a series of curves ar- ranged according to climate, those from regions with cold win- ters and cool summers being at the top, and cool winters and hot summers at the bottom. The curves range from the Adirondacks in northern New York to Tampa in southern Florida, and in- clude one from Denmark. With them I have repeated some of Figured. Human g-S'S&^g-gsltiSS Activity and the ^^^feS^^^^^^O 25 Seasons yT "X . 98 I \ A. Gain in Weight of 1200 Tubercular 90 Patients at Sara- A 1 \ nac Lake, N. Y., g5 1898-1902. 80 \ \ ^^ s ^ S / s J ^ \ 100 98 B. Work of 160 Fac- / x^, s tory Operatives in s S Connecticut, 1913. S" -S Repeated from ** / Figure 1. V /* \^y r\ 98 \x f \ ' f~* C. Work of 60 Fac- % tory Operatives in f ^s ^ ^ ^ V A Connecticut, 1911. 94 Repeated from Figure 2. G D. Deaths in the X / ti , / \ 90 95 State of New York, j / 100 1892-1906. In- D verted. In the scale of this curve, v y 1 / - 105 100 represents the Ss ^ - .** \ j average death w " 110 rate. /-> E. Strength of 90 110 School Children in ^ , / V / \ Denmark, 1904-6. X / ^^ / 105 X ^-S / \ - 100 /~\ / / \ \ 98 f \ / ^/ \ 96 F. Work of 410 Fac- -J ^ / \ tory Operatives in / 94 Connecticut, 1910- p / 1913. Repeated y ^/ from Figure 1. \ /" - 92 100 -\ ^. 98 - / J \ - 90 G. Work of 65 Girls at % Winston-Salem, N. Q ^ /* \ r j % ^ C., 1914. ^^^ * f^ \ / 11 ,Q & c * , b *ti+ J > g Figure 2b. Human ^feS*-*' tories at Columbia, ^ ' ./ J 96 S. C., 19 12- J 4. ^f^ 94 x" ^^ / /^^ \ I. Work of 120 Opera tives in Cotton Fac- w-X v^x v f tories near A u- \, / /* V 100 gusta, Ga., 1912-14. / \ no J. Work of 57 Carpen- ^-\ ^ ters at Jackson- \^ x \ S -. - 96 ville, Fla., 1911 14. ^ ^ ' \ \ ^^. ^ / c ^ \ K. Work of 400 Cipir- \| makers at Jackson- \ I ville, Fla., 1911. e/^ J I ^ ^ 1913, 1914. \ I v > "\ R' X J r- I/ ' ~ A 100 98 96 L. Work of 2300 Cigar- makers at Tampa, \ Fla.. 1912-14. V ** 94 r^ / , 92 \/ *^~ ' 90 NOTE. In Figures 2a and 2b the unit is a year's work of an individual. Thus "120 Opera- tives, 1912-14," means an average of 40 per year for three years. 94 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the curves of Figure 1 for the sake of comparison. The most remarkable feature of this series is that, although there is great diversity of place and of activity, all the curves harmonize with what would be expected on the basis of Figure 1. The first curve, A, is based on the work of Lawrason Brown, a physician who has published records of the weight gained by patients suffering from pulmonary tuberculosis at a sanatorium at Saranac Lake in the Adirondacks. A gain in weight in this disease is a favorable symptom, for one of the most marked effects of tuberculosis is to cause a wasting away of the flesh. In the present tabulation the patients who lost weight are not included, and a drop in the curve does not indicate loss of weight but merely a decreased rate of gain. If the patients who lost weight were also included, however, the form of the curve would still be the same, according to Brown. The Adirondacks, as everyone knows, have long cold winters, while the summers are delightfully bracing, being warm enough to be pleasant, but never hot enough to be debilitating. Hence, from about the first of April to the end of September the sick people make a marked gain. During the other six months, although they may gain more than would be the case in their own homes, they do not find the climate nearly so advantageous as in summer, and the disad- vantage increases until the snow disappears. The next curve, B, is a repetition of the Connecticut curve for 1913. That year the winter was by no means so severe as is ordinarily the case in the Adirondacks. Hence, the curve does not remain low quite so long as does A, and does not begin to fall so soon. The summer, however, was almost as cool as among the Adirondacks, and hence there is no drop during July. The next pair of curves represents a year with a hot summer in Connecticut, C, and the death rate for fifteen years in the state of New York, D. The curve for deaths has been turned upside down, so that high places represent few deaths, that is, high vitality corresponding to high energy in the factory opera- EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 95 lives. In New York State as a whole the effect of the summers is very different from what it is in the Adirondacks. The cities swelter for a few weeks in July, and that sends the death rate up enormously, especially among children, who are quickly taken sick, and who either die after a few days' illness, or re- cover. That is why the curve drops so sharply in mid-summer. In the winter, on the contrary, although it drops almost equally low, the maximum number of deaths per day does not come till March, although by that time the average energy of operatives lias risen considerably. This is because people become sick in January and February, especially those who are elderly, and finally die after lingering illnesses quite unlike those of children. The death rate of other places might be used quite as well as that of New York. The Japanese rate, for instance, is as fol- lows, the figures being those for the ten years beginning with 1899. The figures represent percentages of the normal. Those for the state of New York, computed on the same basis, are added in parentheses : January, 104 (105) February, 108 (108) March, 103 (109) April, 90 (106) May, 85 (100) June, 84 (90) July, 97(110) August, 116 (104) September, 118 (97) October, 102 (89) November, 96 (86) December, 98 (93) Here the course of events is almost the same as in New York, but with significant differences which harmonize with the cli- mates of the two places. Winter in Japan is less severe than in New York, and its effects do not last so long, for the highest mortality is in February instead of March. The Japanese sum- mers, on the contrary, are characterized by prolonged heat, and also by great humidity, especially during the rainy season from July to September. At the end of this period the mortality is at a maximum. The debilitating effect of the summer lasts so long that November and December have a higher death rate than May and June. The late spring is especially favorable, not only 96 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE because of its own excellent character, but because it follows a winter which is not severe enough to be highly disadvantageous. Curves E and F represent the strength of ninety school chil- dren in Copenhagen as measured by Lehmann and Pedersen, and the average energy of factory operatives for four years in Con- necticut. The Danish measurements were carried on during the school years of 1904-1905 when sixty children were tested weekly, and 1905-1906 when ten were tested daily. By com- bining the two years into one and making allowance for the fact that children grow stronger from month to month just as factory operatives grow more skillful, we obtain curve E in Figure 2. Since neither summer nor winter is especially severe in Denmark the dip at the two seasons is the same. The maxima in June and November are almost synchronous with those in Con- necticut. The minima are both delayed six or seven weeks, but the winter minimum in March agrees with the maximum death rate in New York. The summer minimum ought possibly to come in July or August, but the figures for those months are not ob- tainable, for during that time the schools in Copenhagen have vacation. In addition to this we should expect the Danish curve to lag a little behind that of Connecticut because of the maritime climate. Inasmuch as Denmark is constantly swept by west winds from the ocean it does not so quickly grow cool in winter nor warm in summer as does Connecticut, where the prevailing winds are from the continental interior, which of course becomes rapidly warm in summer and cool in winter. Thus it appears that the strength of Danish children and the energy of factory opera- tives in Connecticut have an almost identical relation to sea- sonal variations of climate. Judging by curves C to F in Figure 2 one might hazard the hypothesis that man is subject to a seasonal rhythm which re- peats itself wherever he goes without regard to the climate. On this basis one would expect maxima of efficiency in June and November in all parts of the world. In curves A and B, however, EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 97 we have already seen that where the summers are particularly favorable and the winters unfavorable this rhythm breaks down, and the June maximum and summer minimum disappear. If we go farther south to places where the winters are favorable and the summers very hot, we find a change in the opposite direction, for the winter minimum tends to disappear, and the summer minimum greatly increases and shoves the two maxima more and more into the winter until the two coalesce. This is evident in curves G to L. These represent variations in the wages of piece- workers in southern factories, compiled according to the method used in Connecticut. Curve G shows the work of sixty-five Anglo- Saxon girls in a tobacco factory in Winston-Salem, North Caro- lina. They were pasting labels on cans. Notice how their winter minimum comes in early May instead of June. In September the curve drops suddenly. This is because at that time the effect of the war began to be felt, the price of cotton fell so low that the South was in great distress, and the sale of the goods made by this factory began to be curtailed. Therefore the girls were not given as much work as they could do. Curves H and I are from cotton mills in South Carolina and Georgia, and each represents two mills. In South Carolina the two mills are close together at Columbia, while in the other case they are fifteen or twenty miles apart, one being in Georgia near Augusta, and the other across the Savannah River in South Carolina. The operatives in all cases are of pure Anglo-Saxon stock, chiefly of the "poor white" class. Men and women are included in nearly equal numbers. Part are weavers, while others, engaged in the occupations known as slubbing, spooling, and speeding, tend machines which spin the thread and wind it on bobbins ready for the weavers. In all cotton factories the air in the weaving room, and to a less extent in the others, is kept at a high temperature and a high humidity. This is necessary be- cause when the air becomes cool, or especially when it becomes dry, the thread is apt to break and cause blemishes in the cloth. 98 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Hence, in factories where high-grade goods are manufactured the inside temperature is so abnormal and the amount of goods produced depends so largely on the breakage that it is almost impossible to obtain satisfactory figures. In the factories here considered, however, nothing but coarse cloth is manufactured. The breaking of the thread does little harm, and relatively slight attention is paid to the temperature and humidity of the weav- ing rooms. Moreover, for slubbing, speeding, and spooling, the temperature and humidity make far less difference than for weaving. Hence, the variations in the amount of goods produced per person depend largely on the energy of the operatives in watching their machines and preventing them from standing idle because of broken threads, empty bobbins, or other acci- dents. The exigencies of business, that is, the demands for goods, make no difference to the operatives so far as their production per hour is concerned, for the machines run at a uniform speed whether the factory runs one day a week or six. The cotton mill curves arc essentially the same as that of the tobacco factory. In H there is a double spring maximum, due to accidental cir- cumstances, but the true maximum would probably come about the end of April. In I the spring maximum comes still earlier, that is in mid-April, as is appropriate to a place so far south. The autumn maxima, on the other hand, come later than in Connecticut, one being in early December and the other toward the end of November. The work of carpenters in Jacksonville, as shown in curve J, is different from anything else that is here considered because it is performed out of doors. The fifteen men per year whose records are here used were engaged in making the same kind of repairs time after time. A careful record of the hours that they spend is kept, but the number varies greatly on account of the weather. If it rains they cannot work. Summer is the rainiest period, but that does not tend to diminish the amount of work done per hour. In fact it increases it. The rain comes in hard EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 99 showers, and while it is falling the men rarely try to work, and the time is not reckoned. When the rain is over they work better than before because the air is cooler, although still far from being cool. In winter, on the contrary, from December to March, the rain is a pronounced hindrance. It often comes in the form of a drizzle, and the carpenters try to keep on working while it is falling. Moreover, after the rain the wood is wet, there is apt to be a chilly wind, the hands feel numb, and everything is op- posed to great efficiency. Yet in spite of this, more work per hour is done in February, the worst winter month, than in May, June, July, or August. If these men were at work in well-protected sheds which were heated on the occasional cool days, there is little doubt that in December their curve would reach a maxi- mum higher than that now reached in November, while even if the following months were not still better, they would at least show no pronounced drop. The lower two curves, K and L, represent the work of cigar- makers at Jacksonville in northern Florida and Tampa in the southern part of the state. Those in Jacksonville were mostly Cubans, nearly two thirds being negroes, and the rest of Span- ish descent. At Tampa only a handful of negroes is included, but a large sprinkling of real Spaniards is found among the Spanish Cubans. The curves for the cigar factories are com- piled on a different basis from the others. The reason is that there are no definite hours. The factories are open twelve hours a day, usually from 6 a.m. to 6 p.m. The operatives saunter in as they please, provided they do not come later than 8 a.m., and leave when they choose, although an attempt is made to let no one depart before 4 p.m. While at work they sit close together at tables, and talk volubly except when a hired reader is vocifer- ating the news from a Spanish newspaper. At some time in the morning they go out for a lunch, but are rarely gone as much as half an hour. Otherwise they stay at their work till it is finished. Since there are no fixed hours, we cannot measure the exact 100 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE earnings per hour, as we have done in other cases, but only the earnings in proportion to the time that a man might have worked if he had chosen to do so. In other words, we measure partly the actual capacity for work, and partly the inclination to work. In general the two seem to vary together, but the work of the New York State Commission on School Ventilation has shown that during short periods of high temperature the ca- pacity may remain unimpaired, while the inclination declines. In the practical work of life a lack of inclination is almost worse than a lack of capacity. During the warmer half of the year the possible working time in the Florida cigar factories may be properly reckoned as eleven and a half hours. In winter, however, the light at morn- ing and evening is not adequate for the somewhat exacting work of cigar-making. Therefore the men are not allowed to begin so early as in summer, nor to work so late. The exact time depends on the degree of cloudiness as well as the height of the sun. The factory managers say that in December the working time is cur- tailed an hour and a quarter or more for the month as a whole. In order not to make the winter production appear unduly large, I have reckoned that during the shortest week -not month o the working time is an hour and nine minutes, that is, 10 per cent less than in summer. Before and after that date it steadily increases to the solstices, when it reaches the normal. Thus we get the lower curve for Tampa. It drops low in summer and rises to a single maximum in winter. At Jacksonville the varia- tions in the length of the working day on account of light are less than at Tampa because a lower grade of cigars is made, and hence the men are allowed to work under less favorable condi- tions of light. Inasmuch as the exact effort of dark mornings and evenings cannot be determined, I have drawn two lines at each end of the curve. The lower shows the wages if no allow- ance is made for light, and the upper if the full Tampa allow- ance is made. The actual truth lies between the two. For our EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 101 present purpose this uncertainty makes no difference, since in either case we have the summer minimum and winter maximum which all our other studies would lead us to expect in this lati- tude. The exigencies of business have more effect on the work of the cigar-makers than on that of the other operatives employed in Figure 2, but they do not determine the main fluctuations of the curves here used. In some cigar factories, to be sure, if business is slack the employees are often not allowed to make more than half or two thirds the usual number of cigars. For this reason I have omitted two factories whose figures I worked up, but whose curves I finally found to be almost wholly controlled by the supply and demand of the business. In the three factories which were finally used, however, that is, one at Jacksonville and two at Tampa, the operatives are only rarely placed on a limit. It is too expensive, especially where high-priced cigars are made, for four cigars a day have to be allowed to each man for "smokes." Each man smokes his full number, if not more, no matter whether he makes one hundred cigars or two hundred. The rush season for cigars begins in June or July and becomes increasingly intense until about the middle of November, by which time most of the Christmas orders have been received. Business is dullest in January and February. The operatives, however, know nothing about this, except as they see that men are taken on or discharged. The frequency of changes in the number of employees makes the cigar-maker's life hard, and accounts for much of his proverbial shiftlessness. Another thing which affects the wages of cigar-makers is the dampness of the air. During the warm, damp days so character- istic of the Florida summer, the tobacco is very pliable and easily worked, while on dry, winter days its brittleness causes it to break so that the work is hampered. If it were not for this the difference between summer and winter would be intensified. The most striking proof of the effect of the seasons is yet to 102 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE be recorded. It consists of a series of data corresponding to those of the Connecticut factories, but based on the work of operatives in a large factory engaged in making electrical apparatus at Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. The employees whose wages were investigated were employed in winding wire coils, assembling the parts of motors, and other similar operations which demand accuracy and speed. The admirable way in which the records of this company are kept renders the figures of great value, but lack of time and funds has made it necessary to limit the present investigation to monthly, or, in 1912, bi-weekly averages of hourly earnings. For this reason the resulting curves, which have been inserted as fine dotted lines in Figure 1 (page 59), are smoother than those of Connecticut where the daily earnings have been utilized. The number of piece-workers on which these Pittsburgh curves are based is shown in the following table: 1910. Approximately 950 men and girls in winding section. 1911. Approximately 750 men and girls in winding section. 1912. Twenty-seven girls, winders; 42 men, tinners, black- smiths, painters. In this case all the operatives were especially steady hands who worked throughout the year. In the few cases where they were absent, interpolation has been resorted to. Hence this year's curve is more reliable than the others which are based on all the operatives in a given section or in the whole factory without regard to whether they worked steadily. 1913. Approximately 7000 men and girls in the entire fac- tory. The general form of the curves for Pittsburgh and Connec- ticut is obviously the same. In 1910 notice the deep dip in Janu- ary, and the moderate drop in summer. The next year, 1911, presents quite a different aspect. Because of the hot summer, the depressions in January and July are almost equally deep, the difference between the highest and lowest points is less than EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 103 in most years, and the autumn maximum does not rise high above that of May or June, as is usually the case. The curves for 1912 both show a deep depression in winter which lasts unusually long. During the summer, on the contrary, there is not so great a decrease in efficiency as during the previous two years. Finally, in 1913, both curves rise almost steadily from midwinter to late fall, with only a slight drop in summer. The agreement between the curves for Connecticut and Penn- sylvania is far too close to be accidental. At Pittsburgh, just as at the other factories, variations in the total number of em- ployees form an accurate measure of the demand for work, but these by no means vary in harmony with the actual production per operative. Often the average amount of work done by a given group of individuals, or by all the piece-workers, declines when the number of operatives increases, but quite as often the reverse is true. Hence the conditions under which the factories are run do not explain the variations in wages. Moreover, it stands to reason that the same irregular variations would not occur season after season in an electric factory in Pittsburgh and in brass and hinge factories in Connecticut 400 miles away unless all were under the same control. The only common con- trolling factor which varies in harmony with the curves of Figure 1 is the general character of the seasons. This is essen- tially the same in both places. We have now seen that from New England to Florida physi- cal strength and health vary in accordance with the seasons. Extremes seem to produce the same effect everywhere. The next question is whether mental activity varies in the same way. Leh- mann and Peclersen made a series of tests of the ability of school children in addition. Their general conclusion is that mental work varies in the same way as physical, but reaches its highest efficiency at a lower temperature. This agrees with the investi- gations of a few other scientists, and with the general conclu- sions of the world as summed up in the old adage, "No one is 104i CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE worth a tinker's dam on whom the snow does not fall." Before we can accept this, however, tests are needed on a large scale. The most feasible method at present seems to be by means of the marks of students in such schools as West Point and Annapolis. There the young men live an extremely regular life with a mini- mum of outside distractions. Their recitations are graded with great severity and regularity, and a given subject is often taught six days in the week. The marks are handed to the heads of departments at frequent intervals and are posted where the students can see them. No class is taught in divisions of more more than ten or twelve, so that every student has a full oppor- tunity to show how well he is prepared. In order to avoid all chance of favoritism the instructors do not keep the same divi- sion month after month, but change every few weeks. Altogether it would be hard to devise a system which more thoroughly eliminates the human and accidental factors. As an instructor at West Point put it: "We are not really teachers. We are just put here as officers to see whether the cadets have studied their books, and to decide how many marks to take off." This is pre- eminently true in mathematics, where the solution of a problem is either right or wrong and can be marked accordingly. When I broached my plan to the superintendents at the two academies, it was received with much interest, and every facility was placed at my disposal. I take this opportunity to express my warm appreciation of their courtesy. Some of the instructors were commissioned to see that the proper records were available. The marks of individuals were, of course, not necessary. The various marks for each day or week were merely added, and averaged. The data here employed embrace the following: (1) The weekly averages in mathematics for the first-year or enter- ing class at Annapolis for the six academic years beginning with 1907-1908 and ending with 1912-1913. These classes recite six times a week. (2) The daily marks for the first-year class in English at Annapolis for the year 1912-1913. This class recites EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 105 four times a week. (3) The daily marks in mathematics for a year and a half for the classes entering West Point in 1909 and 1910. Recitations are held six days a week. The classes at Annapolis average about 220 in number and those at West Point about 120. The entire number of students whose marks have been used is between seventeen and eighteen hundred, but as some of the marks cover a period of a year and a half, the total is equivalent to about nineteen hundred students for a single year. All these marks have been combined into the three lower curves of Figure 8. Before discussing them a few words should Oct Nov. Dec, Jan Feb. Mar flpr. Hay June 470 Operative* Z40 Student* in Mathematics at West Point 22O Students in English at Annapolis MOOS Mathematics at Annapolis Figure 3. Seasonal Variations of Mental Compared with Physical Activity 106 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE be said as to the method of preparation. The systems of mark- ing at the two academies are quite different. At Annapolis the department of mathematics tries to keep the average as nearly uniform as possible. If the instructors discover that the average is rising or falling they mark more severely or leniently to counteract it. At West Point, on the other hand, the marks regularly begin high at the opening of the term and fall steadily toward the end. There is no attempt to keep them at a uniform level, but the instructors merely mark harder and harder or give more and more work as time goes on. Both systems tend to mask the effect of the seasons. The influence of the deliberate attempt to keep the marks at a uniform level at Annapolis is largely overcome by using a series of six years. The irregulari- ties of one year counteract those of another except where special circumstances such as vacations interpose a disturbing element at the same time each year. In the English department at Annap- olis there is less stringency about keeping the marks at a uni- form level, and those of a single year show clearly the normal seasonal trend. At the end of the year, however, I have omitted the two weeks before examinations because there was then a sudden spurt accompanied by abnormally high marks. Other- wise all the Annapolis marks without exception have been em- ployed in computing the curves of Figure 3. At West Point it has been necessary to eliminate the effect of the steady fall. The method is the same as in the correction for increasing practice. In order to eliminate the effect of such things as football games, holidays, examinations, reprimands, or other circumstances which clearly have nothing to do with climate, I have omitted all the days whose marks fall more than 10 per cent above or below what would be expected at that par- ticular date. Omissions of this sort are such a common procedure in astronomical and physical measurements that the mathema- tician requires nothing more than a mere mention of what has been done. To the layman it may seem that they are of great EFFECT OF THE SEASONS 107 importance. In reality they rarely alter the general form of the final curves, for exceptionally high figures balance exceptionally low. In the second curve of Figure 3 the effect is slight except upon the first weeks in January. There the minor maximum which occurs just after the Christmas recess is only about half as large as it would be if no data were omitted. At Annapolis it is not necessary to omit the days of special events because the marks are not subject to such wide fluctuations. It is interesting to notice that the classes in mathematics there are influenced by the vacation, which comes at the end of January, just as at West Point. The English marks, on the contrary, are uninflu- enced, probably because English is an easier subject than mathematics. Moreover, as it is taught fewer days per week, and hence has less weight in determining the final marks for the work of the whole year, the students do not devote so much energy to it. By this time the reader has doubtless interpreted Figure 3 for himself. The upper line is the standard average curve for factory operatives in Connecticut. It is the same as the average curve of Figure 1, except that it begins in September instead of January. It is placed here to permit a comparison of the physi- cal work with mental. The curves of mental activity all resemble it in having two main maxima, in fall and spring. At West Point, where the climate is essentially the same as in Connecticut, the mental maximum in the fall comes about ten days later than the factory maximum, while the spring maximum comes two and a half months earlier. Both occur when the mean temperature is a little above 40 F. At Annapolis the maxima are, as it were, pressed toward the winter. The fall maximum in English, to be sure, begins early in November, but lasts till the middle of De- cember. Since it represents the work of only a single year, it is less important than the curve of mathematics, whose fall maxi- mum does not come till the first half of December. The spring maxima of both curves come in the middle of March. At Annap- 108 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE oils, just as at West Point, the time of best work is when the mean temperature is not far from forty degrees. Summing up the matter, we find that the results of investiga- tions in Denmark, Japan, Connecticut, Pennsylvania, New York, Maryland, the Carolinas, Georgia, and Florida are in harmony. They all show that except in Florida neither the winter nor the summer is the most favorable season. Both physi- cal and mental activity reach pronounced maxima in the spring and fall, with minima in midwinter and midsummer. The con- sistency of our results is of great importance. It leads to the belief that in all parts of the world the climate is exercising an influence which can readily be measured, and can be subjected to statistical analysis. It justifies us in going on with confidence to ascertain exactly what effect is produced by each of the climatic elements, such as temperature, humidity, and pressure. CHAPTER V THE EFFECT OF HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE HAVING seen that both physical and mental energy vary from season to season according to well-defined laws, let us now investigate the special features of seasonal change which are most effective. Temperature is far the most important, but before considering it, let us discuss those of minor importance. One of these is light. Many students have ascribed great influ- ence to sunlight, and to its variations from season to season, or from one part of the world to another. For example, C. W. Woodruff, an army surgeon, has written an interesting book on The Effect of Tropical Light on White Men. Its main thesis is that the backwardness of tropical countries is due to excessive sunlight. The actinic rays at the blue end of the spectrum, especially those beyond the limits of vision, possess great chemi- cal power, as is evident from the fact that by their aid photo- graphs can be taken even when no light is visible to the naked eye. Such rays, when they fall upon the human body, are thought to stimulate the cells to greater activity. At first this is bene- ficial : if it goes to excess the cells apparently break down. The process is analogous to the ripening of fruit. A moderate change in the green tissues produces the highly favorable condition of ripeness: more brings on decay. Thus while the return of the light after the winter of the temperate zone may be beneficial, excessive light may be highly injurious. So far as our factory operatives are concerned, no effect of light is to be discerned in the South, while in Connecticut it is at best only slight. The heavy line next to the bottom in Figure 110 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE 1 (page 34) shows that from mid-September to the middle of November the amount of work increases, although the days are growing shorter. This is exactly opposite to what would be expected if the shortness of the days were of primary impor- tance. Moreover, in June when the days are longest we find a sudden drop. If the length of the days had much to do with the matter, there is no reason why more work should be done in November than in June. Nor should we find that a shortening of the days during September is accompanied by the same kind of increase in efficiency which is seen in March when the days, although of the same length as in September, are growing longer instead of shorter. For all these reasons we assign only slight importance to variations in the amount of light. Nevertheless, some effect can apparently be detected. Compare the two lower curves of Figure 1. In spite of the low efficiency occasioned by the winter's cold, the curve of work begins to rise sooner than does the curve of temperature which is placed below it. The first appreciable lengthening of the days in January may cause this by its cheering and stimulating influence. The line of reasoning applied to light applies also to the pos- sibility that the variations of the curve of work depend on the extent to which people are shut up in the house. Obviously, this has nothing to do with the two maxima in November and May, nor with the minimum in July. In November people's houses have been shut up for a month, more or less, while in May and July they are wide open, or at least as wide open as they ever are. The extremely low minimum in January, however, is prob- ably due at least in part to the necessity of shutting up the house in winter. In October the weather becomes so cold that people begin to shut up their houses ; they live in stuffy, unventilated quarters, and fail to take exercise in the open air. By the middle of November this has had time to produce an effect which natu- rally becomes more and more marked as the weeks go on. This would harmonize with the decline of energy from November to HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 111 the middle of January. In January, however, the decline ought not to cease if it is due chiefly to confinement within the house. It ought to continue until about the middle of March, for not till that time do people in Connecticut begin to let in the outside air, and not even then to any great degree. As the curve of work has risen distinctly by that time, some other factor must inter- vene, presumably the increase of light to a slight extent, and the rise of the temperature to a larger extent. A third factor to be considered at this point is the relative humidity of the atmosphere. A sharp distinction must be drawn between the humidity of the outside air and that which prevails within doors. Physicians, students of factory management, school superintendents, and many other people have repeatedly discussed the supposed harmful effects of the dry air in our buildings during the winter. A much more fully attested fact is the harmful influence of great humidity during hot weather. We are more conscious of this than of the harm arising from excessive dryness. This does not necessarily mean that the total effect is worse than that of dryness, however, for hot, humid days are much rarer than the winter days when the air in our houses is drier than that of the majority of deserts. So far as our curves of work are concerned, humidity does not seem to be responsible for the fluctuations except as it is influ- enced by temperature. In other words, the average humidity of the outside air from season to season does not vary in such a way as to cause maxima in May and November, and minima in Janu- ary and July. The average humidity of the outside air in No- vember and in January is not greatly different. Nevertheless, the inside humidity is probably an important factor in causing the low efficiency of midwinter. The relation of work and humidity among the factory opera- tives of Connecticut is illustrated in Figure 4. There the year has been divided in three parts: (1) winter, (2) spring and autumn, and (3) summer. In each part all the days having a 112 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE given humidity have been averaged together, and the smoothed results have been plotted. The heavy, solid lines represent what I believe to be the true conditions when other disturbing elements are removed, while the dotted lines show the actual figures. In winter the dampest days are unmistakably the times of greatest efficiency. We may shiver when the air is raw, but we work well. 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 100% Rel. Hum. 100% 99 98 97 100 99 100% 99 98 97 Figure 4. Relative Humidity and Work in Connecticut a. Winter. b. Spring and Fall. c. Summer. This is partly because in winter the dampest third of the De- cembers, Januaries, etc., averages nearly 2 F. warmer than the driest third. Moreover, moist air at any given temperature feels warmer than dry and hence is less likely to cause people to overheat their houses. In the spring and fall, when the temperature ranges from freezing to 70, with an average of about 50 F., the best work is performed with a relative humidity of about 75 per cent. In other words, neither the dry nor the wet days are the best. The HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 118 summer curve is the most complex of the three. It rises first to a maximum at 60 or 65 per cent, then falls, and once more rises to a higher maximum. The first maximum seems to be due to humidity, the second to temperature. A hot, damp day is un- questionably debilitating. The majority of the dampest days in summer, however, are comparatively cool, for they accom- pany storms. The coolness counterbalances the humidity, and people's efficiency increases. Hence, we disregard the right-hand maximum and conclude that with an average temperature of 65 to 70 a relative humidity of about 60 per cent is desirable. The most unmistakable feature of the curves as a whole is that they show a diminution of work in very dry weather. This presumably has a bearing on the low level of the curve of energy in winter. At that season the air in our houses ought to have a humidity of 60 or 65 per cent, but most of the time the figure is only 20 or 30. On very cold days the percentage is still lower. For instance, if the outside air has a temperature of 14 F. ( 10 C.) and contains all the moisture it can hold, which is usually not the case, its relative humidity when it is warmed to 70 F. will be only 12 per cent. Even on days when the outside humidity rises to 100 per cent and the temperature is 40, the air in an ordinary steam-heated house has a relative humidity of only 85 per cent, which is far below the optimum. Apparently, this extreme aridity is debilitating. It probably dries up the mucous membranes in such a way as to increase our suscepti- bility to colds. In this way it may be an important factor in causing February and March to have the highest death rate of the year. There has been a good deal of discussion as to the actual importance of atmospheric humidity, and no small amount of disagreement. We shall return to the matter later when we study health. While the effects of light, of closed houses, and of excessive dryness explain part of the fluctuations of the curve of work, they have little bearing on any season except the winter. An- 114 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE other matter which may be suggested in this connection is vaca- tions. These, like many other conditions of human life, are largely seasonal. Do people work fast in the fall because they have been rested by vacations? In professional occupations and in business this certainly seems to be the case, but not among factory operatives. As a rule such people do not take summer vacations. They usually stop work at irregular intervals, or else after Christmas when many factories shut down or work on part time for a few days to prepare for the new year. The form of our main curve, however, shows that neither at this time nor in summer do vacations produce any appreciable stimulating results. If they were the cause of fast work, the curve ought to be the highest within a few weeks after the people return to work, but this is not the case. During the vacation period of July and August the amount of work is moderately low, and in early January, after the Christmas break, very low. At the end of August it begins to increase, and increases steadily for two and a half months. The maximum in November is so long after the vacation period that it can hardly have anything to do with it. What has just been said has an important practical appli- cation. There is a common idea that people need vacations in summer. Of course there are strong arguments for this, since pleasant recreation is then possible out of doors. Nevertheless, the need is apparently greater in winter than in summer. To meet this it is probably wise that work should be light during the winter. Already, as everyone knows, many factories run on part time during the first few weeks of the year, and now we see that there are strong physical reasons for this. Another impor- tant suggestion afforded by our curves is this : If the operatives of a factory, or people engaged in any other kind of work, are to be speeded up, the time to do it is when nature lends her aid. To speed up at the end of January is analogous to taking a tired horse and expecting him to win a race. Later in the year, however, during the spring, especially in May, people may ap- HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 115 parently be pushed to the limit, and will not suffer, because their energies are naturally increasing. This is still more the case in October and early November. After the middle of November pressure may produce important results, as we see at Christmas. Nevertheless, the chances are that if continued it will produce undue exhaustion, followed by a serious reaction. Possibly the nervousness of Americans is due partly to the fact that although we relax somewhat in summer, we keep ourselves at high pressure through the winter when the need of relaxation is greatest. Turning now to temperature, we see that in Figure 1 (page 84) the lower curve, showing the march of temperature through the year, and the Connecticut curve just above it are similar in many ways. Both are low in midwinter. From February onward they rise together until about the middle of June. Then the effi- ciency curve falls while the other goes on rising, a condition which fully accords with ordinary experience. The fall of the efficiency curve begins when the average temperature has risen to about 68. When the temperature stops rising, the work stops falling, and then remains nearly steady through July. At the end of July the mean temperature has fallen to about 71. Dur- ing the succeeding period of favorable temperature the two curves disagree, for the amount of work goes up while the tem- perature falls. When the average temperature falls below 48, however, and begins apparently to be unfavorable to physical exertion, the curve of work turns downward. Thereafter, if we omit the Christmas hump and use the dotted line, the tempera- ture and the amount of work decline together until they reach the lowest point in January. It is worth while once more to call attention to the somewhat surprising fact that in southern New England, contrary to our ordinary opinion, low temperature seems to be much more injurious than high. This by no means indicates that high temperature is favor- able. Let us consider the effect of the high temperatures of the four successive summers shown in Figure 1 (page 84). Compare x/fJvy&Jvty August Sept Figure 5. Average Weekly Temperature During the Summers of 1910-13 in Connecticut HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 117 the summer dip in the Connecticut curve, that is, the area below the horizontal lines, with the heavily shaded areas of Figure 5, which shows the average temperature each week during the four summers from 1910 to 1913. The black portions indicate weeks having an average temperature night and day of over 73. The size and distribution of these periods of extreme heat are in close correspondence with the amounts by which the curves of Figure 1 drop below the horizontal lines during the summers. This is illustrated in the following little table. The line marked "defi- ciency in work" indicates the amount by which the efficiency of the operatives diminished because of the hot weather, that is, the area below the horizontal lines of Figure 1. The year when the diminution was greatest is reckoned as 100 and the others in corresponding ratios. The other numbers show the area of the heavy black shading in Figure 5 and represent the intensity and duration of the hot weather. Here, too, the year of maxi- mum heat is represented by 100, and the others by proportional values. YEAR. 1910 1911 1912 1913 Deficiency in work, 58 100 8 2 Severity of heat, 52 100 50 34 In each case 1911 stands highest, 1910 next, and then 1912 and 1913. In 1911 the heat not only was extreme, but lasted long, three weeks at one time and two at another. The death rate for July, 1911, in Massachusetts was 50 per cent greater than in the preceding June. In 1910 the hot weather was not so severe, it lasted four weeks instead of five, and was divided into three parts instead of two. In 1912 the number of hot weeks was the same as in 1910. One was extremely hot, but the rest were not bad. Moreover, they did not come together, and the last was separated from the others by three cool weeks during which people had time to recover, which was not the case in 1910. 118 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Finally, 1913 was a very mild year with only two extreme weeks which were separated by three moderate weeks. An examination of Figure 5 makes it clear that only the ex- treme weeks are harmful. Thus 1911 was a truly terrible summer and 1913 a delightful one. Yet during 1911 the temperature remained above 69 for only eight weeks while in 1913 it re- mained above that figure for twelve weeks. Thus it appears that if the average temperature does not rise above about 70, and if the noon temperature rarely exceeds 80, the physical capac- ity of European races in the United States does not suffer any serious diminution. A slight further rise however only four or five degrees produces disastrous consequences. A single week of such weather does no great harm, but when several weeks come together people rapidly become weakened. The weakening is greater than appears in our diagrams, for during hot spells many of the operatives, particularly the girls, stop work entirely or stay at home in the afternoon. Those who remain are the stronger ones, and naturally their wages are higher than the general average. Moreover, in 1911 the heat was so intense that the factory shut down for two or three days. Thus, if allowance is made for these facts, the difference which a few degrees make between two summers such as 1911 and 1913 becomes even more pronounced. The full effect of a hot summer, especially when it is very damp, may be gauged by the death rate in Japan (page 95). September is there 18 per cent worse than the average, in- stead of 3 per cent better as in New York. The relation between the temperature and the amount of work in winter during the four years under discussion is not so pro- nounced as in summer, but can easily be detected. The hot sum- mer of 1911 was followed, as frequently happens, by an uncom- monly cold winter. The reason for both is the same. Usually hot weather in New England is commonly due to the movement of heated air from the interior toward the coast, particularly from the southwest to the northeast. Cold winters are due to a similar HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 119 transportation of air from the interior, this time from the north- west. The interior of a continent, as is well known, cools off very rapidly in winter and becomes hot rapidly in summer. When these conditions are carried from the interior to the coasts, they bring to New England what climatologists call a continental climate instead of the more maritime climate which otherwise prevails. The effect of the cold winter of 1911-1912 can easily be seen in the curve for 1912 in Figure 1 (page 84). That year the aver- age temperature where the factories are located was 19.0 for the first five weeks compared with an average of 32.7 for the three other years whose curves are given. For the next five weeks the temperature was 24.4 compared with 35.3. The effect of this is seen in the low position of the 1912 curve of work far into the spring. The fact that the energy of the operatives remained low after the temperature began to rise suggests that the eifect of extreme conditions may last long after more normal condi- tions begin to prevail. The same thing is suggested by the fact that after the summer of 1911 the curve of work does not rise so high in November as in the preceding May. During each of the other three years the November maximum is higher than its predecessor. Although a single winter and a single summer are not enough to prove that the effect of extreme conditions does thus persist for many months, they suggest that a long stay in an adverse climate may produce results which last for years. In spite of a previous statement, it appears that our plan of es- caping from possible extreme heat by taking summer vacations in the mountains or at the seaside is wise. Equally wise is the growing habit of getting away from the severe cold for a while in winter. The only trouble is that those who most need such a change are rarely the ones who get it. If people could spend the summer on the Maine coast, the winter in Georgia, and the rest of the year in New York, they ought to be able to do the best Mon. Tve. Thvr Fri. J 4- 5 ^^ -**. e I00 98 <& 97 Figure 6. Effect of the Days of the Week on Piece- Workw 1. 60 Men, April-July, 1912. 2. 60 Men, August-November, 1011. . 49 Girls, 1912. 4. 31 Men, 1012. 5. 14 Girls, 1912. 6. 24 Men, 1912. 7. 60 Men, January-March, 1910. 8. Weighted Average of Nos. 1-7, or approximately MO People for On* Year* HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 121 kind of work at all seasons almost without the necessity of a vacation. The effect of temperature ma}' be shown in more ways than have yet been presented. Let us determine how fast people work on days having various temperatures, no matter in what month they occur. The very cold days, of course, all come in winter, but may be in December, January, or February. The very hot days come anywhere from May to September, while days with a temperature of about 50 occur in almost every month of the year. The method can be illustrated by taking all the Mondays, all the Tuesdays, the Wednesdays, and so forth, and averaging the work of each day of the week. This has been done for 230 people. The results are shown in Figure 6, which is inserted to show exactly how our results are obtained, and how necessary it is to have a large number of people. We are striving to separate the effects of one single condition from those of a vast number. We start with the wages of individuals which vary from day to day for hundreds of reasons wholly unconnected with the day of the week or the weather. The variations are so great that even if a man is influenced by the approach of pay-day, for example, we should probably not be able to detect it if we merely looked at his wages for a month or two. Therefore we average all the people of a department together, and obtain results such as appear in Figure 7. This shows the actual wages in percent- ages of the maximum which were earned by 170 people divided into five departments during five weeks in January and Febru- ary, 1913. There is little uniformity in the different lines. Where one goes up the other goes down. Yet closer examination shows that in at least four out of the five departments the wages during the last two weeks were a little larger than during the earlier weeks. The variations of the different curves are in part due to the persistence of individual vagaries which have not yet been averaged out, and in part to conditions affecting whole depart- 122 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ments. For example, a foreman is cross one day and good- natured the next ; a belt breaks and delays work ; or some of the operatives converse so much that their work suffers appreciably. If a number of departments are averaged together these acci- January Monday Monday Monday February Monday Monday 7. Variations in Daily Wages. Five Departments (170 people) at New Britain, Conn. HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 123 dents, as well as those which pertain to individuals, disappear, but not until a great many people are considered. To find the effect of the days of the week, we take data such as are illustrated in Figure 7, select all the Mondays, Tuesdays, and so forth, and average each day. This gives the curves of Figure 6. Here we begin to detect a certain degree of uniformity, although the accidents and peculiarities of each department are still in evidence. On the whole, however, the curves are higher at the end of the week than at the beginning. All, to be sure, are irregular, and the two lower not counting the heavy line slope in the opposite direction to the rest. The fact that the re- maining five slope in the same direction shows, however, that these different people in different factories and during different years were subject to a common influence. Finally, we average all the departmental curves, giving each a weight proportional to the number of operatives. Thus we obtain the heavy lower line of Figure 6. This is still irregular, for although 230 people are included, all influences other than that of the days of the week are not yet eliminated. Nevertheless, the wages clearly in- crease toward the end of the week. If the operatives were paid by the day instead of by the piece, this would probably not be the case. They would work slowly at the end of the week by reason of being tired. With the piece-workers, on the contrary, other considerations are dominant. If they work a trifle slowly on Monday, they can make it up tomorrow. On Tuesday they can be slow and make it up on Wednesday, but a few who fell behind on Monday are beginning to work harder. So it goes from day to day until on Friday and especially Saturday many feel that their earnings for the week are insufficient, and hence make an extra effort. In some cases this may not be true, as in the curve next above the average curve. Yet it remains a general truth, and the lower curve of Figure 6 is a concrete expression of the fact that in the factory under discussion there is a difference of at least 2 per cent between Monday and Saturday. Possibly 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 - 4-1 Figure 8. Human Activity and Mean Temperature A. 300 Men in Two Connecticut Factories, 1010-13. B. 196 Girls in One Connecticut Factory, 1911-13. C. One Man (P) in Denmark, June-December, 190(1. D. One Man (L) in Denmark, June- December, 1906. E. 880 Cigar-makers in Factory B at Tampa, Fla., 1913. F. 400 Cigar-makers in Factory A at Tampa, Fla., 1913. G. 3 Children Typewriting in New York, 1905-6. H. 880 Cigar-makers in Factory B at Tampa, Fla,, 1912. I. 1560 Students in Mathematics and English at West Point and Annapolis, 1909-191& NOTE. All the curves except G and 1 every case is reckoned as 100. The maximum ill HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 125 the real difference is greater, and is obscured by other circum- stances. In the cigar factories of Florida it rises to a far greater value, for the Cubans are much disinclined to work after a holi- day. Not only are about 10 per cent of the operatives absent on Mondays, but those who are present come so late or are so in- disposed to work that they accomplish only about 80 per cent as much work as on other days. This is so important a matter that allowance for it has been made in computations where in- dividual days rather than weeks are concerned. The figures for each day of the week for 780 men at Tampa are as follows : Monday, 81.9 per cent; Tuesday, 98.7 per cent; Wednesday, 99.8 per cent; Thursday, 100 per cent; Friday, 98.3 per cent; and Saturday, 97.9 per cent. The other days are reckoned as of equal weight, but the figures for Monday have been increased in the ratio of 82 to 100. By the employment of a method similar to that used with the days of the week we obtain the curves shown in Figure 8. These are based on varying numbers of people, from one to over 700. Yet all show the same general character. With the exception of G and H, which are distinctly the least reliable, the physical group all reach maxima at a temperature between 59 and 65. Even the two less reliable curves reach their maxima within the next four degrees. All the curves decline at low temperatures, that is, on the left, and also at high. The irregularities at the extreme limits are largely due to the fact that there the number of days is so small that exact results cannot be hoped for. Figure 8, with the brief statements which accompany the respective curves, tells the whole story so plainly that it scarcely seems worth while to amplify it. Several points, however, may well be emphasized. For instance, below a certain temperature, which varies from curve to curve, a further reduction does not seem to produce much effect. People apparently become some- what hardened, or else the conditions within the warmed houses do not change much in spite of a change in the outside air. An- 126 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE other noticeable thing is that the curve for girls has greater amplitude than that for men in the same region. Part of this is due to the inclusion of the group of Italians, already referred to, who are engaged in drawing hot brass and hence are benefited by the coldest kind of weather. Even if they were omitted, how- ever, the girls' curve would still vary more than that of the men. This seems to indicate that either because of their sex or because of their age, girls are more sensitive than men. Another point brought out by the curves is that as we go to more southerly climes the optimum temperature of the human race becomes higher. It is important to note, however, that the variation in the optimum is slight compared with the variation in the mean temperature of the places in question. For instance, in Connecticut the optimum seems to be about 60 for people of north European stock. This is about ten degrees higher than the mean temperature for the year as a whole. In Florida, on the other hand, the optimum for Cubans is about 65, which is five degrees lower than the mean temperature for the year at Tampa. In other words, with a difference of twenty degrees in the mean annual temperature, and with a distinctly northern race com- pared with a southern, we find that the optimum differs only about 5 F. This seems to mean that for the entire human race the optimum temperature probably does not vary more than ten or fifteen degrees. We have not yet pointed out all the important matters sug- gested by the curves of Figure 8. Above the optimum the curves in general begin to decline quite rapidly, but then cease to do so and at high temperatures are not so low as would be expected. This is largely because in hot weather many operatives, espe- cially the girls and the Cubans, do not feel like work, and so stay away from the factories. Those who come in spite of the heat are the strongest and most efficient. Naturally, their aver- age wages are higher than those of the ones who stay away, and hence the general level of our curves is too high in the portions HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 127 based on the hottest weather. The mental curve, however, falls off very rapidly at high temperatures. This is because the stu- dents are obliged to be present on hot days just as on others. They must recite whether they wish or not. Hence, their curve is more reliable than the others. In this connection some ex- periments carried on by the New York State Commission on Ventilation are of interest. In an attempt to determine the most favorable conditions of ventilation the Commission placed sev- eral groups of persons in rooms where the temperature and hu- midity were under exact control, and measured their strength, mental activity, food consumption, and other conditions. The experiments lasted six or eight hours a day, and each set of subjects was tested for several weeks. Three temperatures were used, namely, 68, 75, and 85. No appreciable effect upon strength could be detected, nor upon mental activity, and vari- ous other functions. This is probably because the experiments were not sufficiently prolonged. That is, the subjects were in the experimental rooms only a third or a quarter of each day, and hence their condition did not have time to change appreciably. Although the subjects did not lose in actual strength, however, their inclination to work declined at high temperatures even within six or eight hours. Thus far we have been dealing with large bodies of people. It is peculiarly important to find that no matter how small the number, the same relation to temperature is discernible. One of the curves in Figure 8 shows the speed and accuracy of three children who wrote upon the typewriter a few stanzas from the "Faerie Queen" or a page from George Eliot daily for a year, and weekly for another year. Their records were kindly placed at my disposal by Prof. J. McK. Cattell. I have corrected them for the effects of practice, and have combined speed and ac- curacy in such a way that each has the same weight. At one period, for some unknown cause, the efficiency of the children declined greatly for two months or more. If this were eliminated 128 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE their maximum would come at a lower temperature than now appears, probably not much above 60. In the curves of indi- viduals, we are fortunate in having careful tests made by two psychologists, Lehmann and Pedersen, at Copenhagen. They tested their own strength daily with the dynamometer, and their curves, copied directly from their monograph, are before us. One is uncommonly regular with a maximum at 64<. The other, less regular, has its maximum at 59. The agreement of Danish curves based on single individuals with New England curves based on hundreds is highly important. The last thing to be considered in Figure 8 is the mental curve at the bottom. It is based on so large a number of people, and is so regular, that its general reliability seems great, although I think that future studies may show the optimum to be a few degrees higher than is here indicated. It agrees with the results of Lehmann and Pedersen. Furthermore, from general observa- tion we are most of us aware that we are mentally more active in comparatively cool weather. Perhaps "spring fever" is a mental state far more than a physical. Apparently people do the best mental work on days when the thermometer ranges from freezing to about 50 that is, when the mean temperature is not far from 40. Inasmuch as human progress depends upon a coordination of mental and physical activity it may be that the greatest total efficiency occurs halfway between the mental and physical optima, that is, with a mean temperature of about 50. Curves such as those of Figure 8 are not peculiar to man alone. They are apparently characteristic of all types of living creatures. To begin with plants, many experiments have deter- mined the rate of growth of seedlings at various temperatures. The commonest method has been to grow different sets of seed- lings in large numbers under conditions which are identical ex- cept in temperature, and then to measure the average length of the shoots. In all cases growth is slow at low temperatures, in- creases gradually with higher temperatures, reaches a maximum HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 129 like that of man, and then falls off quickly. The course of events, however, is not always so regular as here indicated. The curve of wheat, for example, as worked out by MacDougal is given in Figure 9. The peculiar double maximum there seen appears in each case where careful tests are made. It seems to be due to some inherent quality of the plant, and is of especial interest in Figure 9. Growth of Wheat at Various Temperatures After MacDougal The figures on the left indicate growth in mm. during 48 hours our present study because we shall soon come upon an analogous case in man. When many species are averaged, such irregulari- ties disappear, and we obtain the curve at the bottom of Figure 10, which has been prepared by MacDougal on the basis of his own measurements and others given in such works as Pfeffer's Physiology of Plants. Many of the lower plants, such as marine algae, have their optima at lower temperatures than those here indicated, and the same is probably true of Arctic species. On the other hand, certain low algae which grow in hot springs must have their optimum at a temperature above that of ordinary plants. These differences are immaterial. We are now concerned 130 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE only with the fact that so far as plants have been measured, their response to temperature resembles that of man. Apparently, we have to do with a quality which pertains to all kinds of living beings, and is presumably an inherent char- acteristic of protoplasm. The nearest approach to pure proto- plasm is found in unicellular organisms whose bodies show only the beginnings of differentiation into parts having separate functions. The infusoria furnish a good example. One of these, paramoecium, has been carefully studied by L. L. Woodruff. His Menial Energy Mental and rriysiccilEnefqy Combined Physical Energy Absorbhon of Oxyoen by Crayfish Raie of fission of infusoria Growth j/a/rts' Figure 10. Mean Temperature and Vital Processes in Plants, Animals and Man HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 131 original purpose was to determine whether it was possible for this organism to keep on reproducing itself without conjugation for any great length of time. Under the conditions of nature the small motile cells often spontaneously develop a median cell wall and ultimately divide into two new individuals, thus reproducing the species. This process, however, does not go on indefinitely, for when two cells come in contact they fuse with one another, and then begin a process of fission which, like the other process, ends in two individuals. Thus we have two types of reproduc- tion, asexual and sexual, which apparently give rise to the same kind of paramcecia. Woodruff's purpose was to determine whether asexual reproduction can persist indefinitely, or whether it leads in time to extinction. He has shown that if the media of nutrition contain a sufficient number of elements, paramcecium can reproduce itself indefinitely by the asexual method. Between May 1, 1907, and May 14, 1914, he had carried his cultures through 4417 generations without conjugation. In the spring of 1924 the paramoecia were still thriving after about fourteen thousand asexual generations. In the course of this work he has found that the rate of cell-division is an accurate test of the conditions under which protoplasm exists. For example, when extracts from nephritic kidneys or certain other diseased organs are added to the nutrient solution, even though they are present in such small quantities that they cannot be detected by chemical analysis, they make their presence evident by a falling off in the rate of fission. One of Woodruff's most important lines of work has been to test the relation of his infusoria to temperature. From many experiments he finds that their activity corresponds closely to van't Hoff's law of chemical activity. According to this well- established law, chemical reactions of most kinds at ordinary temperatures become nearly three times as active with every rise of 10 C. Even in inorganic chemical reactions, however, and far more in those of the living cell, there is a distinct limit where 132 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE this rule breaks down. This limit forms the optimum of the species. At higher temperatures the degree of activity declines, and finally death ensues. On the basis of these conclusions, Woodruff's data permit us to draw the second curve from the bottom in Figure 10. The next higher curve shows the amount of oxygen absorbed by the common crayfish at various temperatures. The most ex- tensive work on this subject appears to have been done by Brun- now. The facts here given are taken from the summary by Putter in his Vergleichende Physiologie. The amount of oxygen ab- sorbed by an animal is an excellent measure of its physical activity. When supplemented by measurements of the amount of carbon dioxide given off, and of the speed with which certain other metabolic or katabolic processes take place, it gives a true picture of the animal's general condition. Apparently, these various processes follow van't Hoff's law just as do the growth of plants and the cell-division of the infusoria. The optimum in the three cases does not vary greatly, that for plants being about 86, for paramoecium 83, and for the crayfish 74 F. Physiologists are not yet fully agreed as to the cause of the phenomena shown in these curves, although there is little doubt as to the general facts that they imply. One hypothesis may be briefly stated. According to Putter's summary, the most prob- able explanation is that activity goes on increasing according to the ordinary chemical law until it becomes so great that the organism is not capable of absorbing the necessary oxygen. That is, at a low temperature the creature easily gets what oxy- gen it needs, and gives it out again in the form of carbon dioxide or of other oxidized products which remove the waste substances from the body. As the temperature rises, the normal increase in chemical activity takes place, the animal is still able to get rid of all its waste products, and thus its life processes are strength- ened. With a further rise of temperature a change sets in. The chemical processes which break down the tissues of the body be- HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 133 come still more active, but the supply of waste products to be eliminated by oxidation becomes so great that they cannot all be removed. This is because in every organism there is a distinct limit to the amount of oxygen which the creature can mechani- cally convey to different portions within a specified time. If the supply of oxygen is not sufficient to oxidize all the waste prod- ucts, some of these will remain in the system. They act as poisons. Their first effect is to diminish the organism's activity. If they accumulate to too great an extent death ensues. The discussion of this hypothesis must be left to the physiolo- gists. They must decide whether the hypothesis which explains the curves of cold-blooded animals and plants is also applicable to warm-blooded animals. There can be little doubt, however, that variations in the rate at which metabolism takes place in the human body play a part in the variations in efficiency which we are here studying. The researches of Thomson illustrate the way in which we are beginning to discover the truth. In Man- chester, England, from April to July, 1910, and again in March, 1913, he measured the percentage of CO2 given off in the breath of four individuals under different conditions of temperature, humidity, and pressure. From his figures, given in the Man- chester Memoirs, I have compiled the following tables : I. PERCENTAGE OF CC>2 EXHALED BY FOUR PERSONS UNDER DIFFERENT CONDITIONS OF TEMPERATURE Temperature, 50-51 52-53 54-55 56-57 58-59 60-61 62-63 64-65 98 4<7r 4 * r2 4t66 42 EXHALED BY FOUR PERSONS UNDER DIFFERENT CONDITIONS OF HUMIDITY Relative Humidity, 70-75% 76-80% 81-85% 86-90% Percentage of CO 2 , 4.75 4.60 4.60 4.45 134 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE The interpretation of these tables is difficult, and I can merely offer a suggestion. An increase in the proportion of CO2 ex- haled from the lungs obviously indicates an acceleration of the metabolic processes which break down and consume the bodily tissues. This liberates energy which may manifest itself in at least three ways and possibly more. It may give rise to heat which is used to maintain the body at the normal temperature ; it may be used to accomplish physical or mental work; and it may cause an excess of heat which gives rise to further metabo- lism of a harmful nature. In the first part of the table the per- centage of C02 is comparatively high at the lowest temperature recorded by Thomson, and decreases with only slight irregu- larity till the thermometer reaches 62 F. This is close to the temperature which we have found to be the optimum. Below that point the increased metabolism is probably needed to keep the body warm. At higher temperatures increased production of CO2 is again apparent. This perhaps means that too much chemical activity is taking place, and that toxic substances are accumulating in the way suggested by Putter. At the optimum, according to this interpretation, the body does not have to use an undue portion of its strength in keeping warm, nor is it injured by too great stimulation. Thus it is in the best condition for work. The second part of the table shows that in the driest weather which England enjoys, metabolism is more active than in wet weather. Perhaps part of this is due to the fact that in dry air the body loses water and is cooled by evaporation, and hence requires more heat than in wet air of similar temperature. There is more to the matter than this, however, but further measure- ments are needed before an adequate explanation can be offered. All that can be done here is to point out the fact that in man, as in the lower organisms, activity varies according to tempera- ture. This is evident in Figure 10, where the dotted upper line is the curve of mental activity, while the accompanying solid HUMIDITY AND TEMPERATURE 135 line shows the conditions if all accidental irregularities could be removed. The third line in the same way represents the physi- cal activity of both men and women in Connecticut. I have not used the figures from the South because they are not quite so reliable as those from Connecticut. Finally, the second line from the top shows physical and mental activity combined, each being given the same weight. It may be taken as representing man's actual productive activity in the things that make for a high civilization. The resemblance of the human curves to those of the lower organisms is obvious. In general, the lower types of life, or the lower forms of activity, seem to reach their optima at higher temperatures than do the more advanced types and the more lofty functions such as mentality. The whole trend of biological thought is toward the conclusion that the same laws apply to all forms of life. They differ in application, but not in principle. The law of optimum temperature apparently controls the phenomena of life from the lowest activities of protoplasm to the highest activities of the human intellect. CHAPTER VI WORK AND WEATHER THE effect of a given climate depends on two primary fac- tors. One is the character of the seasons as expressed in averages such as are furnished by our weather bureaus. The other is the changes from day to day, that is, the weather. The boy quoted by Mark Twain was nearly right when he defined the difference between weather and climate as being that "Cli- mate lasts all the time and weather only a few days." Two climates may be almost identical in their seasonal averages, and yet differ enormously in their effect on life, because in one the change from day to day is scarcely noticeable, while in the other there are all sorts of rapid variations. The old Irish- woman who was driving her pigs to market in a pouring rain did not realize it, but she gave expression to a truth of the greatest importance, when a friend pitied her for being out in such weather, and she replied, "Indade it's bad, but sure it's thankful I am to have any kind of weather." The changes from one day to another depend largely upon our ordinary cyclonic storms. In such storms the barometer goes down and then up ; the wind changes in direction and ve- locity ; the air becomes humid, clouds gather, rain usually falls, and then clear skies and dry air prevail; the temperature also changes, often rising before a storm and falling afterward, although the exact sequence depends on the location of a region in respect to the ocean and to the center of the storm ; the daily range of temperature also varies, for in damp or cloudy weather the nights do not become so cool nor the days so warm as when WORK AND WEATHER 137 the air is clear. To understand the influence of the weather all these conditions must be investigated. Most of them, however, appear to be of relatively slight importance when considered by themselves. For instance, Lehmann and Pedersen could find no appreciable effect of the pressure of the atmosphere except where low pressure prevails a long time. The decrease in effi- ciency at such times, however, is probably due more to pro- longed cloudiness and its attendant circumstances than to the barometric conditions. My own work leads to the same result. The curves of efficiency compared with pressure are so contra- dictory that it does not seem worth while to publish them. The same is true of the range of temperature from day to night, and of the direction and force of the winds. I have no doubt that all these matters are important, and that some day their effect will be worked out. In general, however, their influence is exerted indirectly through changes in temperature and humidity. In hot weather a great range from day to night is unquestionably highly favorable, but at ordinary temperatures it seems to make no special difference, except through its effect upon the mean temperature. As to the winds, Dexter, in his book on Weather Influences, shows that they produce a marked effect upon the nerves, as is indicated by the unruliness of school children in Denver when high south winds prevail. Part of this is doubtless due directly to the wind, but the unseasonably high temperature and extreme dryness which accompany it are probably more important. Yet we are all conscious of the effect of a steady high wind. Some people are stimulated. I have seen a small boy, who was usually very quiet, climb to the top of a tall tree when a violent wind came up, and swing in the branches, singing at the top of his voice. For a while such stimulation is probably beneficial, but if continued day after day it makes people excitable and cross. A striking example of the effect of a prolonged wind is seen in eastern Persia in the basin of Seistan. During the summer, from 138 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE June to September, the so-called "Wind of One Hundred and Twenty Days" blows so violently from the north that in the oases trees cannot grow except under the lee of high walls. The acrid wild melon, which ripens its beautiful little green and yellow fruit in the desert, does not spread its slender branches in all directions after the common fashion of plants. The gales crowd the branches into a sheaf which points so uniformly in one direction, a little to the west of north, that it can safely be used as a compass. When Europeans have to endure this wind they say that it is one of the most trying experiences imaginable. Not only does it render them irritable, but it deadens their ini- tiative and makes them want to stay idly in the shelter of the house. The natives, although possessed of many good qualities, are inert and inefficient even in comparison with their fellow Persians who live farther to the north and west. On the whole, we may probably conclude that occasional short-lived gales and frequent light or moderate winds are beneficial, while long periods either of steady calms or of gales are depressing. Aside from the conditions of weather already mentioned, there are two whose effect appears plainly when curves are con- structed according to the method described above. One is the change of temperature from one day to another, and the other is the character of the day as to clouds and sunshine. In con- sidering changes of temperature from one day to the next, we deal with the mean temperature for each day and not with the extremes. A change of as much as 15 is rare. Suppose that the thermometer stands at 60 at sunrise, rises to 80 by two o'clock in the afternoon, then falls rapidly to 50 at sunset and to 40 by midnight. Suppose also that the next day the tempera- ture is 40 at sunrise, rises a little above 55 during the day, and falls again to 45 at night. The two days would be very different, and we should speak of them as being marked by a very great change of temperature, a difference of 40 within ten hours. Yet the average of the first day would be about 64 and WORK AND WEATHER 139 of the second 49, a difference of only 15 in the mean tempera- ture. On the basis of this supposition the reader can estimate the importance of the various degrees of change indicated in Figure 11. At the left the curves show the average efficiency on days when the temperature has fallen ; in the middle are the days with no change ; and at the right are the days characterized by a rise. Taking only the two upper curves, those for men and girls in Connecticut factories, the resemblance is striking. When we con- sider the heterogeneous character of the original materials the resemblance is still more important. The men's curve is based on 120 men at Bridgeport in 1910 and 1911, and on 180 men at New Britain in 1911, 1912, and 1913. The girls' curve is based on 196 girls at New Britain in 1911, 1912, and 1913, and on 60 girls at New Haven in 1913 and 1914. Even when the girls and men are working in the same factory, there is no reason, aside from the weather, why their wages should be high on the same day. The chief difference between the two curves is that the one for the girls varies more than that for the men, and reaches its maximum slightly farther to the right. Apparently, here, just as in the case of mean temperature, the girls because of their age or sex are more subject to the influence of the weather than are the men, and hence their curve dips deeper. Let us now interpret the upper curves, beginning at the middle. There they fall to their lowest level. This means that when the temperature of today is the same as that of yesterday, people work more slowly than after a change, no matter whether the change is upward or downward. A variable climate is there- fore highly desirable if people are to be efficient. Perhaps the most surprising feature is that the lowest point of the physical curve, and a depression of the mental curve, C, come not at 0, but at 1. The zero point is low, lower than any point of the physical curves except 1. Hence, our conclusion as to the injurious effect of uniform temperature is justified, but that Fall of Temperature -14F -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 +2 Rise of Temperature "H +6 +8 +10 100% 98 100 99 97 100 99 Figure 11 Human Activity and Changes of Mean Temperature from Day to Day A. 300 Men in Two Connecticut Factories, 1910-13. B. 259 Girls in Two Connecticut Factories, 191 1-18. C. 460 Students in Mathematics and English at West Point and Annapolis, 1900-1911 D. 760 Cigar-makers at Tampa, Fla., in Winter (October-March), 1912 and 1013. Factory A. E. 400 Cigar-makers at Tampa in Winter, 1913. Fac'- B. F. 400 Cigar-makers at Tampa in Summer (April-September), 1913. Factory B. G. 380 Cigar-makers at Tampa in Summer, 1912. Factory A. H. 880 Cigar-makers at Tampa in Summer, 1913. Factory A. A B -M 100 100 WORK AND WEATHER 141 does not explain the curious dip at 1 . The repetition of the same phenomenon in each of the three upper curves, and a simi- lar occurrence at 2 and 3, respectively, in the two curves for the winter in Florida strongly suggest that we are con- fronted by a peculiarity which pertains to man as a species, in the same way that a double optimum of mean temperature per- tains to wheat as shown in Figure 9. Possibly, a slight fall in temperature causes people to shiver, as it were, and only when the fall is slightly larger is the circulation of the blood so stimu- lated as to increase the activity of the various organs. In the South it may be that people's blood is more sluggish than in the North, so that the reaction due to cooler weather does not follow quite so soon, and hence the period of shivering is not over until the fall in mean temperature amounts to more than about 3. I do not assert that this is so, but it is the only expla- nation that comes to mind. To go on with our interpretation of the physical curves, a slight rise of temperature seems to be favorable, but beyond that the favorable effects of increased heat, which are strong in cold weather, are neutralized by the unfavorable effects in warm weather. In fact, our personal experience tells us that even when the heat is not extreme, a sudden rise may make us un- comfortable and lazy, as often occurs in the spring. In spite of this, however, a rise is in general better than uniformity. When the temperature falls, on the other hand, a distinct stimulus is received, provided the fall amounts to as much as 4. The best effects are seen with a fall of from 6 to 9 with girls and of 7 to 11 with men. Here again the implication is that men are on the whole less sensitive than girls. An extreme drop is not so favorable as one of more moderate dimensions, especially for the girls. Taking the physical curves as a whole, the greatest amount of energy would be expected in climates where the mean temperature first rises 2 or 3 a day for a few days and then drops 4 to 8 a day. If the changes are greater than this, the 142 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE effect is still stimulating, but not so beneficial as under the more moderate conditions. If there is practically no change, on the contrary, the level of efficiency lies within the low central de- pressions of our curves, and is less than under either of the other conditions. Mental work resembles physical, but with interesting differ- ences. When the temperature falls greatly, mental work seems to suffer more than physical, and declines as much as when there is no change. It receives a little stimulus from a slight warming of the air, but appears to be adversely affected when the air becomes warm rapidly. This last statement, however, must be qualified. The physical curves are based on the complete year, and the conditions of summer have an opportunity to balance those of winter. The results show the net effect for all seasons combined. The mental curves, on the other hand, do not include the summer vacation, which lasts from the middle of June to the first of September at West Point, and from the middle of May to the first of October at Annapolis. If this were included, the effect of a pronounced lowering of the temperature would be more noticeable than at present, for such a lowering is naturally more stimulating in July than in January. In another respect, also, the curve of mental efficiency needs modification. It is based on figures from two climatic provinces, namely, southern New York and Maryland. The great decline at times when the tem- perature rises rapidly is due largely to conditions in Maryland, where the hot days of the spring are much more debilitating than in New York. The students belong to a race which has never learned to endure sudden heat. Hence they feel it strongly. If allowance is made for the two conditions just mentioned, the mental curve will approach much more closely to the physical. A drop of temperature amounting to 8 or more will appear more stimulating than now seems to be the case, and a rapid rise will not seem so harmful. Hence, the general conclusion for both physical and mental activity will be essentially the same. WORK AND WEATHER 143 It may be summed up thus : Taking the year as a whole, uni- formity of temperature causes low energy ; a slight rise is bene- ficial, but a further rise is of no particular value ; the beginning of a fall of temperature is harmful, but when the fall becomes a little larger it is much more stimulating than a rise ; when it becomes extreme, however, its beneficial qualities begin to decline. This conclusion must, of course, be appropriately modified ac- cording to the season. A cold wave in January is very different from one in July. In our curves we have given January and July an opportunity to neutralize one another. They have not done so. This means that after all allowances have been made for the seasons, the total effect of cold waves is decidedly beneficial, and of warm waves slightly so. Frequent changes, therefore, are highly desirable. Let us pass on now to the Florida curves. Here we find a curious difference between summer and winter which is not easy to understand. Let us leave that for the moment, however, and consider only the two winter curves. Their general resemblance is marked. The differences at the extremities are not important because the number of days there concerned is very small. It must be remembered that the two curves are from independent and rival factories. The position of any particular point in either curve depends upon a number of days scattered irregularly through the months from October to March. Aside from a genu- ine effect of climate, there seems to be no possible way in which 400 men in one factory in 1913 could be made to work so that their curve would be the same as that of 380 men in another factory in the two years 1912 and 1913. Here, as in Connecticut, West Point, and Annapolis, we are apparently dealing with a peculiar quality which is inherent in the human species. One of the Florida curves, E, is low at 0, while the other is medium. This means that days when there is no change of temperature are not particularly favorable. At plus 2 to plus 4, however, both are fairly high, which indicates that a moder- CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ate rise of temperature is favorable. A further rise seems to be harmful. The effect of a slight fall of the thermometer has al- ready been discussed. A further fall is beneficial. The most notable thing about curves D and E is the maximum from 4< to 7. It comes at about the same place as the mental maxi- mum, and is similar to the Connecticut maximum except that the people in the far South do not seem to be able to stand such extreme changes as do those in the North. In fact, it seems most significant that the Connecticut men, who are the strongest of our various groups, are most stimulated by a strong change of temperature. The Connecticut girls come next, but, being less sturdy, they do not profit quite so much by rigorous conditions. The mental curve is largely determined by Annapolis, and as the climate there is less severe than in Connecticut, the students seem to feel more keenly the effects of extreme changes, although they are stimulated by those of moderate dimensions. The same is still more true of the people of Florida in winter. Finally, during the summer the Floridans are stimulated by a slight drop of temperature, provided it is not enough to make them feel chilly, but enough to start their blood in motion. A greater drop makes them feel cold, while even the slightest rise of temperature in their long monotonous summer is unfavorable. We are ready now to sum up our results. The outstanding point is that changes of temperature, provided they are not too great, are more stimulating than uniformity, while a fall is more stimulating than a rise in the latitudes now under consideration. The effect of changes depends largely upon the degree to which people are inured to them. When they are weakened by a long hot period like that of the Florida summer, even a slight cooling of the air brings relief and activity, provided it does not go so far as to make people feel chilly. When the same Floridans become wonted to the somewhat sterner, albeit mild air of their winter, the first effect of a lowering of the temperature may be to make them shiver, but soon they are stimulated, and work fast. They WORK AND WEATHER 145 are not so tough, however, as to be able to get benefit from the occasional days when really strong cold waves sweep down upon them. On the other hand, a rise of temperature stimulates them, unless it is of considerable severity. Farther north the same applies except that, being tougher, the people are more benefited by strong changes. Judging by the difference between summer and winter in Florida, it looks as if a little hardening would cause even the Cubans to respond favorably to changes at least as severe as those in Maryland, thus making the left- hand part of their curve like C in Figure 11. Taking it all in all, the one thing that stands out preeminently is that a fall of from 4 to 7 is everywhere stimulating, provided people are accus- tomed to it. Man is not the only organism that is benefited by changes of temperature. Numerous experiments have shown that plants are subject to a similar influence. If a plant is subjected to un- duly low or high temperature, its growth is retarded. As the temperature approaches the optimum, the rate of growth in- creases. When the optimum is maintained steadily, however, not only does the increase cease, but retrogression sets in, and the rate of growth declines. A moderate change of temperature away from the optimum and then back again after a few hours checks this decline, and keeps the plant at a maximum degree of ac- tivity. Thus conditions where the thermometer swings back and forth on cither side of the optimum are distinctly better than where the optimum is maintained steadily. Thus it seems to be a law of organic life that variable temperature is better than uniformity. The physiological process by which frequent changes of tem- perature affect the body is not yet known. The best suggestion seems to be that of Dr. W. B. James. It is universally recognized that one of the most important of the bodily functions is the circulation of the blood. The more active and unrestricted it is, the more thoroughly is the whole system nourished and purified. 146 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Provided it does not impose an undue strain on the heart or arteries, anything that stimulates the circulation appears to be helpful. Changes of temperature are a powerful agent to this end. Witness the effect of a bath, either cold or very hot. Few things are more stimulating than a Swedish bath. An attendant holds two hoses, one with cold water and the other with hot, and plays them alternately upon the patient. A man goes into such a bath with hanging head and dragging feet. He comes out with head erect and a new spring in his walk. Apparently, frequent changes of the temperature of the air produce much the same effect. No one change produces so pronounced an effect as a Swedish bath, but the succession of stimuli due to repeated changes throughout the year must be of great importance. Before leaving this subject, let us test the effect of changes in still another way. Let us see what happens during an average series of days such as make up our common succession of weather in New England. The ordinary course of events is first a day or two of clear weather, then a day or two of partly cloudy weather, next a cloudy day with or without rain, and finally another cloudy day during which rain falls. Then the sky clears in prepa- ration for another similar series. On this basis I have formed the six groups indicated at the top of Figure 12. At the left, the efficiency on all clear days which follow cloudy or partly cloudy days has been plotted, just as in another diagram we plotted the efficiency on Mondays. Next come the clear days which follow another clear day. If several of these follow in unbroken suc- cession, they are all included, but a third or fourth clear day is rare. In the next group come the partly cloudy days which follow either a clear or a cloudy day. The great majority follow clear days. A second partly cloudy day is much rarer than a second clear day, and a third is still rarer. The first cloudy day, the fifth column, includes cloudy days which follow either clear or partly cloudy days. Finally, the sixth column includes not only the second cloudy day, but the third and fourth if such are WORK AND WEATHER 147 recorded. In general, this column represents days when a storm comes to an end, while the one to the left of it represents the time when a storm first becomes well established. The rest of the diagram, to the right of the sixth column, is merely a repetition of the part already described. It is inserted to show how an ideal series of storms would repeat itself. Figure 12 discloses some surprising facts. For instance, the first clear day is characterized by the slowest work in the two 100% 99 100 100 99 Figure 12. T'ie Stimulus of Storms (1) 60 Men at Bridgeport, 1910. (2) 60 Men at Bridgeport, August, 1911-July, 1912. (3) 170 Men and Girls at New Britain, 1913. (4) Weighted Average of (1), (2), and (.!), Ktiual to 2PO People for One Tear. U) (2) (3) (4) 148 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE upper curves and by almost the slowest in the third. Our im- pression of the stimulus of the bright, clear air after a storm receives a flat contradiction. It is apparently psychological, not physical. The second clear day makes a better showing than the first. It stands high in two curves, and low in only one. The first partly cloudy day is high in one curve, and medium in two. The second partly cloudy day is medium in all three. The same is true of the first cloudy day. The last cloudy day is as sur- prising as the first clear day. In each of the three curves it stands highest. People work fastest at the end of a storm. In the lower curve of Figure 12, the whole matter is summed up in a single line. Here we see that during an average "spell of weather" people are least efficient on the clear days ; moderately efficient on the partly cloudy days, and on the first cloudy day ; and most efficient at the end of a storm. We may tell ourselves that this is unreasonable, but when we think it over, we are likely to be aware of its truth. Before a storm we may feel depressed, but at the end, when the rain or snow is almost over and the air begins to have that excellent quality which makes us forget all about it, we bend to our work with a steadiness and concentration which are much less common at other times. Hellpach empha- sizes this in his book on the psychological effect of geographical conditions. We fail to appreciate it largely because the aesthetic impressions of a beautiful, clear day are felt much more con- sciously than are the physiological conditions which throw us vigorously into our work. Each storm, with its changing skies, varying humidity, and slow rise and rapid fall of temperature, is a stimulant. Each raises our efficiency. This ends our survey of the effect of climate upon daily work in the eastern United States. We have considered the influence of the seasons, of mean temperature, of humidity, of winds, of changes of temperature from day to day, and of the character of each day and its relation to storms. We have also seen that although different races, or people under decidedly diverse cli- WORK AND WEATHER 149 matic environments, are at their best at slightly different tem- peratures, the differences are inconsiderable, and changes of temperature are as valuable to one as to the other. The question now arises whether the climatic effects are really of great im- portance. In Figure 12, the stimulus of the succession of clear and cloudy days amounts to only 1 per cent. In Figure 11 changes of temperature from day to day produce a variation of only a little over 2 per cent, if we omit the irregular and un- reliable extremities of the curves. In Figure 4, the maximum effect of humidity appears to be only 3 per cent. In Figure 8, however, the differences are greater, for the effect of mean temperature upon the girls in Connecticut is 7 per cent. Finally, in Figure 1, the effect of the seasons reaches nearly 9 per cent when four years are averaged, and nearly 15 per cent for indi- vidual years. These figures are far from representing the full importance of the various factors. This will readily appear from a little consideration. In the preceding paragraph, the percentages in- crease in proportion to two conditions, first, the degree to which the influence of a single factor is separated from the influence of all other factors, and second, the length of time during which each factor is able to exert its influence. The smallest figure, 1 per cent in Figure 12, does not represent any individual factor, unless it be cloudiness. It does not even represent the fluctua- tions which attend an individual storm, for the days were selected without regard to their position in a cyclonic disturbance, but simply according to their cloudiness. The variations shown in the curve are due to many factors, including mean temperature, changes of temperature, relative humidity, and others of minor importance. As no two of these are necessarily at their maximum at the same time, they neutralize one another. Moreover, a given condition lasts only a day in most cases, and so has no oppor- tunity to produce any great effect. In the curve of changes of temperature from day to day, which shows the next larger 150 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE effect, a single factor is singled out. Its full force can by no means be seen, however, for the humidity often varies in such a way as to neutralize it. Moreover, the effects of especially low or high temperatures may often completely overshadow any stimulus arising from the mere fact of a change. Furthermore, the effect of changes of temperature rarely continues more than two days. For example, if the thermometer averages six degrees lower on one day than on the preceding, it may happen that there will be a further drop before the next day, but there is far more chance that the temperature will rise a little or remain station- ary, or fall so little that it will not be stimulating. Hence, the effect is rarely cumulative, and the influence of each single day must usually stand by itself. Much the same is true of relative humidity, except that by heating our houses we artificially in- duce long periods of great aridity. The effects of mean tempera- ture, on the other hand, have greater opportunity to show their full importance, though they, too, are hampered. Relatively low or high temperatures last many weeks, which makes it possible for the effect of day after day to accumulate. Yet our curves by no means show the full effect, for a cold day with a mean temperature of 30 may come in November at a time when effi- ciency is still at its highest. It produces its normal effect, but a single unpropitious day, or even a week, does not suffice to de- press people's vitality to a degree at all approaching the low limit reached after two months of cold weather. Likewise, a day with the most favorable temperature, not far from 60, may be sandwiched between very hot days in July, or between two cold days in March. Hence, people will display little energy on those particular days, and the average efficiency at the optimum tem- perature will appear correspondingly lower than it ought. Finally, the seasons have more opportunity than the individual climatic elements to produce their full effect. Even here, how- ever, the variability of our climate does not allow any special combination of circumstances to work long unimpeded. Warm WORK AND WEATHER 151 waves break the cold periods of winter, and cool waves come in summer. Storms are more active in winter than in summer, and hence their stimulus works toward overcoming the effect of pro- longed cold. Moreover, no single season is of great duration, and extreme conditions do not last long enough to produce their full effect. From all this we may conclude that the total influence of climate upon energy is much greater than appears in any one of our curves. The difficulty of determining the exact proportions of any individual influence may be made clear by an example. We know that man's power to work depends upon food, drink, sleep, and clothing. Suppose that while he was still supplied with these in normal quantities we were to try to measure the effect of each. We should test his strength at stated intervals after he had eaten his meals, or after he had had a drink. We should find out how many hours he slept each night and compare that with his work. We should measure his achievements before and after he put on his spring underwear or fall overcoat. We might get re- sults, but it is highly doubtful whether they would be as distinct as those here discussed. We have no difficulty in measuring the effect of food, drink, sleep, and clothing, for we can easily vary them to suit the needs of our experiment. With climate the case is different. We must take it as we find it, and must experiment on people who are constantly subject to its influence. Some day we shall test people first in one climate and then in another, but that will be difficult because it takes a considerable time for cli- mate to produce its full effect. Being obliged to search for the effects of climate without being able to change them in accord- ance with the needs of our experiment, we are in almost as diffi- cult a case as the experimenter who should desire to determine the effect of the amount and kind of food consumed by a group of individuals, but who had no control over how much they ate. They might allow him to measure what was set before them at each meal and what remained when it was over, but they would 152 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE eat as much as they liked and when they liked. He would get results, if he did his work carefully, but they would by no means represent the full effect of food. The influence of climate upon men may be likened to that of a driver upon his horse. Some drivers let their horses go as they please. Now and then a horse may run away, but the average pace is slow. Such drivers are like an unstimulating climate. Others whip their horses and urge them to the limit all the time. They make rapid progress for a while, but in the end they ex- haust their animals. They resemble climates which are always stimulating. In such climates nervous exhaustion is likely to prevail and insanity becomes common. A third type of drivers first whip their horse to a great speed for a mile or two, and then let them walk slowly for another mile or two. They often think that they are accomplishing great things, and they are better off than the two types already mentioned, but they still have much to learn. They are like a climate which has a strong con- trast of seasons, one being favorable and the other unfavor- able. Still a fourth kind of driver may whip his horse sometimes and sometimes let him walk, but what he does chiefly is to urge the animal gently with the voice, then check him a little with the rein. By alternate urging and checking he conserves the animal's strength, and in the long run can cover more distance and do it more rapidly than any of the others. Such a driver resembles a climate which has enough contrast of seasons to be stimulating but not to create nervous tension, and which also possesses fre- quent storms whose function is to furnish the slight urging and checking which are so valuable in the total effect, although each individual impulse is almost unnoticeable. CHAPTER VII HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE* WE have investigated the relation of the weather to human energy. Let us do the same for health. In a previous chapter we saw that variations in the death rate in New York and Japan and the gain in weight among tubercular patients exhibit seasonal fluctuations much like those of workers in fac- tories. As a means of measuring health, deaths are more im- portant than disease for two reasons. First, experience has shown that when several years are averaged together, the death rate is an almost perfect measure of the number and severity of the diseases which afflict a community. Second, the records of disease are very scanty and imperfect ; they have never been tabulated on any large scale for the entire population. Only in rare cases can the records of certain diseases be used as well as those of deaths. Accurate mortality records, on the contrary, have now been kept for many years. The health of practically every community varies in response to the seasons. In the northern United States most physicians are far busier in February and March than in May and June. In July and August the demand for their services increases again, especially among children. Then come the best months of the year, especially October, when good health and good spirits abound. Different types of disease, to be sure, display different seasonal adaptations. Those of the respiratory organs, for ex- * This chapter and the two that follow are entirely new. They are based largely on publications mentioned in the list at the end of the prefaces, but none of the material in Chapter IX has hitherto been published. 154 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ample, reach a maximum in winter, while those of the digestive tract are more numerous in summer. Admitting, then, that both energy and health show marked seasonal variations, our aim is to determine how closely the two sets of variations are in harmony. Part of the answer is illus- trated in Figure 13. The upper line, A, represents variations in 1010 mi 1*12 1913 J ASOND J FMAM J J A 8 OND J FMAM J J ASOND J F MAM J J AS ON D Figure 13. Seasonal Variations in Energy and Health A. Work of factory operatives in Connecticut. B. Work of factory operatives in Pennsylvania. C. Health (death rate inverted) in Connecticut. D. Health (death rate inverted) in Pennsylvania. Scale for A and B on left, for C and D on right. B and D are placed below A and C for con- venience, although belonging at essentially the same level. the efficiency of factory workers in Connecticut from January, 1910, to December, 1914, as already given in Figure 1. The second line, B, is the similar curve for Pittsburgh. The two lower lines illustrate fluctuations in health in Connecticut (C) and Pennsylvania (D). They are the curves of the death rate in- verted so that high parts indicate good conditions or few deaths, and low parts, poor conditions or many deaths, thus per- mitting easy comparison with the efficiency curves. It is ob- HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 155 vious not only that the two efficiency curves and the two health curves run almost parallel, but that there is also a close parallel- ism between health and efficiency. Aside from the weather all possible causes of such parallelism seem to be excluded, for epi- demics, business disturbances, and the like, did not occur in such a way as to explain the similar fluctuations in two diverse phe- nomena hundreds of miles apart. Note how closely the four curves agree even in details. Low efficiency during January, 1910, is followed in a month or two by very poor health. During the spring both efficiency and health improve. Then comes the summer with a mild tendency toward a drop in all the curves, and the autumn with the main maximum of the year. In 1911 the parallelism of the four curves is again evident, as is the lag of the mortality curve after that of efficiency. In 1912 and 1913 the sag of all the curves in sum- mer diminishes and disappears ; for those years, it will be re- membered, had only short periods of really hot weather. The similarity of the four curves, especially in the summer, would be still more marked were it not that the deaths of children under two years of age have been omitted in the mortality curves. Since many other observations point in the same direction, we conclude that unfavorable weather, such as commonly pre- vails in January, has an immediate effect in reducing people's vitality and energy. Hence their work falls off. At the same time they become more susceptible to disease. Accordingly, in due time the number of deaths increases. Naturally the greatest mortality lags several weeks after the lowest efficiency ; it takes time for bacteria to produce infection, and for infection to lead to death. The lag is longer in winter when respiratory diseases are the chief enemy than in summer when digestive diseases with their more rapid course are the chief foes. The agreement between health and energy is thus so close that both appear to depend npon essentially the same fluctuations of the weather. It is especially important to determine the relation of the 156 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE weather to mental as well as physical health. Hence peculiar interest attaches to certain studies of mental abnormalities carried on by Norbury.* He finds that the admissions to psy- chiatric hospitals show that "Mental disorders in their incipi- ency and recurrence parallel the efficiency curves of Huntington. (Maximum in the spring, minimum in the autumn.)" His curves show that certain maxima of admissions for mental disorders oc< curred at the following periods : Civil Hospitals of New York State, 1916-1921, June. Norbury Sanatorium in Jacksonville, Illinois, 1900-1923, May. Massachusetts State Hospital, 1922-1928, May and June. State Hospitals, northern United States, 1922-1923, March. After the maximum the diminution in admissions is in all cases very rapid. The number of admissions fell to the lowest point at the following periods : New York State Hospitals, September-February. Dr. Norbury's Sanatorium, October-February. Massachusetts State Hospital, November-February. State Hospitals, northern United States, August-February. Across the Atlantic insanity in London reaches a distinct maxi- mum in May and is low from July to February. Almost identical conditions prevail as to suicide, except that the fall from the high point in June is not so rapid as in the case of insanity, al- though the minimum is reached earlier, that is, in November and December, instead of from July to February. As to the nervous disorders in continental Europe, Gamier states that general paralysis in Paris follows a seasonal course almost identical with that of insanity in London. Now nervous breakdowns, in- sanity, suicide, and paralysis, as Norbury shows, are all due * Frank Parsons Norbury: Seasonal Curves in Mental Disorders. Medi- cal Journal and Record, vol. 119, 1924. HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 157 mainly to the same cause, namely fatigue of the nerves. Such fatigue, he says, is apparently controlled to a large degree by the seasons. But just as the maximum number of deaths lags some weeks or even a month or two after the time when the weather produces the lowest efficiency in factories, so the maxi- mum effect of fatigue of the nerves lags still more, and the great- est number of nervous breakdowns may occur three or four months after the period of least efficiency as measured by daily work. The universality with which the bodily functions respond to the seasons may be judged from two other recent investigations. In one case Hess* has shown that among infants the phosphates of the blood, which are an essential element for growth, show a pronounced seasonal tide. During the year covered by his ob- servations the percentage of phosphates in the blood stood at 4.34 mg. per cent during June and July, 1921. It may have risen higher during the succeeding months, but no records were kept. In December it had fallen to 3.92 and in March to 3.58. Pi'esumably it would have fallen still lower, but whenever the phosphates fell below 3.75 the children were treated with ultra- violet light, which effectively increases not only the phosphates, but also the calcium and probably other important elements of the blood. It has been found by many authorities that the chil- dren's disease known as rickets follows a seasonal course like that of the phosphates and is connected more or less closely with the amount of ultra-violet light. For our present purposes, how- ever, the important point is that the essential phosphates in children show the same general seasonal variation as the death rate, and as mental breakdowns among adults, the lag being perhaps greater than in the death rate but less than in mental collapses. "Alfred F. Hess: A Seasonal Tide of Blood Phosphate in Infants. Th* Journal of the American Medical Association, December 30, 1922, vol. 79, pp. 2210-2212. 158 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Another case of seasonal fluctuations is discussed by Porter* of the Harvard Medical School. In cooperation with the Health Department of Boston, monthly records of the weight of several thousand of the youngest school children were begun in 1909 and continued until 1919. Among the boys born in 1905 the average increase in weight from month to month during the years 1911 to 1918 was as follows : January to February -{-0.18 Ibs. July to August -f0.801bs.f February to March -{-0.4/7 August to September -}-0.93t March to April -f-0.22 September to October -f 0.96 April to May 0.16 October to November -f 0.61 May to June -j-0.05 November to December -|-0.63 June to July -fO.SOf December to January -fO.98 In interpreting this table, allowance must be made for cloth- ing. In May the children exchange their winter clothes for those which are somewhat lighter, while about the end of September the opposite change takes place. Allowance must also be made for the long summer vacation with its opportunities for out- door play which naturally causes the children's weight to in- crease rapidly. If allowance is made for these two facts the regularity of the seasonal trend of growth is intensified. From January onward the children grow slowly ; in the spring after the phosphates have reached the minimum and at the very time when grown people are most subject to mental breakdowns, the children practically cease to grow. Not until the summer vaca- tion begins do they recover from the effect of the winter. In the summer the fourfold advantages of freedom from school, favor- able weather, much outdoor life, and a more varied and healthful diet than at other seasons cause rapid gain in weight. This gain * W. T. Porter: The Seasonal Variations in the Growth of Boston School Children. American Journal of Physiology, May, 1920. f The boys were not weighed during the summer vacation. The average increase in weight from June to September was 2.23 pounds, and this has been allotted to the summer months in what seem to be reasonable propor- tions. HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 159 seems to be checked somewhat when school begins, but is resumed during the late fall and early winter. In spite of confinement in school, little outdoor play, and a diet relatively poor in vita- mines and other important elements, the children gain weight more rapidly in December than in any other month except perhaps at the end of the summer vacation. The chief favorable factor appears to be the climate, the stimulus of which does not disappear until the advent of really cold weather. The sudden decline in the children's rate of gain during January appears to correspond closely with the drop in the efficiency of factory workers at about the same time, but the effect on growth lasts much longer than does the more direct effect upon activity. Among the many other instances of seasonal fluctuations in human health, one of the most remarkable is illustrated in Figure 14. The upper line indicates for each month the average Apr May June July Aug Sept Oci Deo Jcu> 200 Conceptions DealHs body Increase of Population Figure 14. Seasonal Variations in Conceptions and Deaths in Japan, 1901-1910 160 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE daily number of conceptions which resulted in the birth of living children in Japan during the ten years from 1901 to 1910. A pronounced maximum in June is followed by a diminution of 46 per cent, which culminates in September at the end of the long, hot, humid summer. Then comes a recovery which is checked but not reversed during the winter, and which resumes its course during the delightful spring weather of April, May, and June. The second curve shows the average number of deaths per day. It is almost exactly the reverse of the curve of conceptions. In other words, during the months when many people are sick and die, the number of conceptions is either very low or else a large number of conceptions result in miscarriages or still-births. The most extraordinary feature of Figure 14 is the fact that the curves of conception and mortality cross one another in Sep- tember. The same fact is illustrated in the lowest curve, which indicates the excess of conceptions over deaths. In June the conceptions which give rise to living children outnumber the deaths by nearly 2.5 to 1. In September the conceptions are less numerous than the deaths. No seasonal variations in farm work or social customs seem competent to explain more than an in- significant part of the great contrast between May and Sep- tember. The explanation seems to be that the hot, humid summer saps the vitality of the Japanese, especially the women, so that they are physically unable to reproduce themselves. If the weather which prevails in July and August should prevail throughout the year, the Japanese as a race would apparently diminish in numbers instead of increasing with disquieting rapid- ity. Under such circumstances natural selection would presum- ably work with great vigor. A race might arise capable of withstanding the most intense tropical conditions, but it would presumably differ from the present Japanese in many qualities such as energy and initiative. It would be easy to multiply examples, but space forbids. All sorts of physiological conditions appear to vary from season HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 161 to season in essentially the same way except that some responses, such as the energy of people in good health, lag only a little after the climatic conditions, others, such as diseases and the rate of reproduction, lag several weeks ; and still others, such as the growth of children and the occurrence of nervous breakdowns and suicides, lag still farther. In the case of the mental disturb- ances the lag is so great that it almost seems as if the onset of stimulating weather after a period of unfavorable weather, had the effect of causing a sudden collapse. In reality the real state of affairs may perhaps be this : the winter months produce an effect like that of driving a horse without rest and as rapidly as possible over a bad road. When a stretch of good road is reached the driver, to whom we may liken the weather, whips the animal to his topmost speed and the tired beast soon breaks down. Let us now try to analyze the effect of the seasons upon health, and determine the relative part played by temperature, humid- ity, and variability. A study of about nine million deaths by means of climographs as described in World Power and Evolu- tion, leads to the conclusion that the optimum or most favorable condition for human health is an average outside temperature of about 64 F. (18 C.) for day and night, a relative humidity of about 80 per cent, and a fairly high degree of storminess, or at least of variability from day to day. This means a climate in which the midday temperature rises to 70 more or less, while that of the night falls below 60. With the rise in temperature the noonday relative humidity declines to perhaps 60 per cent, while during the cool night it rises high enough so that dew is precipitated. But a constant succession of clear days is not desirable. There must be occasional rains and variations in tem- perature, wind, and cloudiness from day to day. Tampa experi- ences such conditions at the end of February, New Orleans in March, Asheville in May, Atlantic City in early June, Seattle in August, Nantucket and Boston in September, and Portland, 162 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Oregon, in October. At the seasons when people go in largest numbers to many famous health resorts the majority of such places enjoy climatic conditions closely approaching those which are ideal for physical health. EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON HEALTH AND STRENGTH D Effect B C of change A No. of Best of 10F. on Nature of criteria persons temperature death rate 1. Deaths, north Italy, 1899-1913 781,000 58 21% 2. Piece-work, men, two Connecticut factories, 1910-1913 300 59 3. Piece-work, girls, one Connecticut factory, 1911-1913 200 60 4. Deaths, British Columbia, 1914- 1916, 8,000 62 25% 5. Deaths, southeastern United States, 1900-1912 122,000 62 10% 6. Piece-work, Pittsburgh, 1910-1913, 9,000 63 __ 7. Deaths, southern Italy, 1899-1913 752,000 63 27% 8. Cigar-making, Cubans, Tampa, Florida, 1913 380 64 9. Deaths, southern France, 1901-1910 838,000 64 17% 10. Deaths, northeastern United States, 1900-1912 2,500,000 64 15% 11. Deaths, whites, eastern United States, 1912-1915 921,000 64 12% 12. Deaths, dry interior of United States, 1900-1912 71,000 64 15% 13. Deaths, northern France, 1901-1910 1,315,000 65 17% 14. Deaths, east central United States, 1901-1910 739,000 65 18% 15. Cigar-making, Cubans, Tampa, Florida, 1913 400 65 __ 16. Deaths, negroes, eastern United States, 1912-1915 167,000 68 12% 17. Cigar-making, Cubans, Tampa, Florida, 1912 380 68 18. Deaths, California, 1900-1912 142,000 70 18% HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 163 The preceding table illustrates the type of statistical evidence on which is based the conclusion that a mean temperature of 64 F. is the optimum. The experimental evidence will be illus- trated later. Columns A, B, and C explain themselves. Column D indicates the approximate percentage by which a change of 10 F. raises or lowers the death rate when the temperature ranges between 30 and 60. When all seasons are taken together the net effect of a rise of temperature under such conditions is to lower the death rate, while a fall increases the death rate. This, however, applies only to a rise or fall in which the new condition of temperature endures for some time, as in the change from season to season. In twelve of the eighteen cases in this table the optimum out- side temperature was from 62 to 65 F., and in five cases 64 F. Two of the cases where the optimum falls to 60 or lower were piece-work in Connecticut factories. This may be because such work involves mental as well as physical activity. We have found some evidence that the optimum temperature for mental work is considerably below that for physical work. Naturally an occupation where mental and physical alertness are both needed would be most favored by a temperature between the best temperatures for the body and the mind. As to the low optimum of the north Italians, 58, I have no explanation. The cause could perhaps be detected by a study of the other elements of the weather such as humidity and wind, or of local diseases such as malaria. It is worth noting, however, that Campani* from an analysis of 24,500 deaths at Milan obtains results closely similar to those here set forth. His results, to be sure, are espe- cially important in respect to variability rather than tempera- ture. He finds that deaths are least numerous just after storms while the wind is blowing. They are most numerous in still air during periods of stagnation and after periods with little change * A. Campani: Oazetta degli Ospeddiedelle Cliniche, Milan. 164 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE of temperature. Changes of temperature are beneficial in north Italy just as in America. Among the three cases of the preceding table where the opti- mum temperature is above 65, one represents Cubans of Spanish descent but with a good deal of colored admixture, and a second represents negroes. In the first case life in a tropical climate has presumably raised the optimum temperature some- what. In the second, although the negroes here dealt with lived largely in the parts of the United States from Maryland north- ward, they probably still retained an ancestral adaptation to a slightly warmer climate than that which is best for the white race. It is worth noting, however, that the Cubans have spent practically their whole lives where the coolest month averages about 70 and the hottest over 80, while the ancestors of the negroes have dwelt for untold generations in regions still warmer. Nevertheless the optimum for both groups, 68, seems to be lower not only than the average temperature of their homes, but than the average for the coolest months in those homes. I might add that for negroes the optimum humidity seems to be a little higher than for white men. This again may be an inheritance from a former environment. Such differences between diverse races suggest that permanent physiological changes take place whereby races become adjusted to diverse climates. The slightness of the differences, however, suggests that such adjustment is very slow and incomplete. An earlier and more fundamental adjustment to climate appears still to be largely dominant, and may represent the climate under which man's chief physical evolution took place. As for the extreme variation in California, where 70 appears to be the best temperature, I am inclined to think that it is due to accident. The California results depend largely on two cities, San Francisco where the mean monthly temperature never reaches 70, and Los Angeles where conditions of wind and humidity may account for the favorable conditions during the HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 165 months of high temperature. It is interesting to note that the average of the six extreme cases in the table is 64.2, against 63.9 for the twelve medium cases. It must be borne in mind, however, that the best temperature is not the same in dry climates as in moist. We shall return to this later. The conclusion that an outside mean temperature of about 64 F. is the optimum for Europeans agrees closely with the con- clusion now widely accepted that an inside temperature not above 68, and preferably lower, is the ideal. The effects of de- viations from this ideal have been the subject of many careful experimental investigations, among which those carried on by the New York State Ventilation Commission under the chair- manship of Prof. C.-E. A. Winslow are especially notable. The results of these experiments, as set forth in a volume entitled Ventilation, are so important that I shall conclude this chapter by quoting several pages which pertain not only to temperature but to other atmospheric conditions. "The work of previous investigators from Hermans to Leon- ard Hill has made it abundantly clear that extreme high atmos- pheric temperatures are highly prejudicial to human health and comfort, and that it is to such temperatures rather than to chemical pollution that the most serious effects of bad air are due. The most important result of our experiments has perhaps been the demonstration that even moderately high temperatures between 24 and 30 C. (75-86 F.) are accompanied by demonstrable harmful results. "A. Body Temperature and Circulatory Phenomena. We find that the rectal body temperature exhibits a definite relation to the temperature of the atmospheric environment, the body temperature when observed at 8 a. m. during the summer time showing a fairly close parallelism with the average temperature of the outdoor air for the preceding night. In our experimental chamber we found that at 68 F. the rectal temperature and heart rate tended to fall, the Crampton index of vasotone in- 166 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE creased, the resistance of the peripheral portion of the circu- latory system rose, and the velocity of the blood flow was correspondingly lessened. At 75 F. the rectal temperature, heart rate, and Crampton index changed but slightly. At 86 F. with 80 per cent relative humidity, the rectal temperature and heart rate rose, the Crampton index fell, the peripheral resist- ance decreased and the velocity of blood flow increased. "The final average results attained under the three atmos- pheric conditions were as indicated below. 20 C. (68 F.) 50 per cent relative humidity 24 C. (75 F.) 50 per cent relative humidity 30 C. (86 F.) 80 per cent relative humidity Rectal temperature Heart rate, reclining Heart rate, standing Crampton index 36.7 C. 66.0 77.0 60.0 37.0 C. 81.0 105.0 45.0 37.4 C. 74.0 99.0 34.0 "We are not prepared to say whether the maximum heart rates attained at 75 F. as compared with 86 F. are significant ; but it is clear that rectal body temperature bears a direct, and the Crampton index an inverse, relation to atmospheric tem- perature. After vigorous physical work the return of the heart rate to normal was somewhat more prompt at 68 F. than at 75. "Systolic and diastolic blood pressure showed no very definite relation to temperature between 68 F. and 86 F. ; but at 101 F. blood pressure as well as rectal temperature and heart rate showed marked increases. "B. Other Physiological Phenomena. The rate of respiration was slightly increased by moderately high temperatures, from an average of 17.9 at 68 F. to 19.3 at 75 F. and 19.7 at 86 F. ; while at 101 F. the increase was very marked. The dead space of the lungs, the volume of the supplemental air, the alkaline reserve of the blood, the respiratory quotient, the car- HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 167 bohydrate metabolism, the protein metabolism and the total metabolism showed no demonstrable relation to atmospheric temperature under the conditions studied in our experiments. "C. Comfort and Mental Efficiency. So far as the sensations of the subjects are concerned, as evidenced by their votes as to the comfortableness of the experimental chamber, the difference between 68 F. and 75 F. is comparatively slight; but a tem- perature of 86 F. with 80 per cent relative humidity is dis- tinctly uncomfortable, the average votes falling sharply when- ever this condition is reached. This discomfort is not, however, accompanied by any inability to perform mental work ; such as naming of colors and opposites, cancellation, mental multiplica- tion, and addition. Subjects urged to maximal performance did equally well under both conditions, the conditions being main- tained for four hours a day on five consecutive days, or for eight hours a day on four consecutive days. Longer hours of exposure continued for a prolonged period might of course yield different results. Even when the subject was left free to work or not, as much mental work was accomplished at 75 F. as at 68 F. although in one short experiment a temperature of 86 F. did seem to diminish the inclination to do mental work. At 75 F. typewriting (which involves a certain amount of neuro-muscular activity) seemed to be slightly diminished while performance in mental multiplication was actually increased. In general, how- ever, we have no clear evidence that moderate overheating im- pairs mental efficiency. "7X Influence of Atmospheric Humidity. Somewhat exhaus- tive studies of the alleged influence of atmospheric humidity upon mental achievement and comfort have yielded entirely negative results. With relative humidities of 50 per cent and 25 per cent, respectively (all other conditions being the same), there was no significant difference in the votes of the subjects as to their subjective sensations of comfort, no difference in temperature of the air next to the chest, or in pulse rate, and 168 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE no difference in the performance of a long series of complex neuro-muscular tasks specially designed to test the alleged in- fluence of a dry atmosphere upon nervousness and efficiency. Longer periods of exposure might of course produce effects not detected by us ; but it seems clear that exposure to a relative humidity as low as 25 per cent at a temperature of 75 F. for eight hours a day on five days a week does not produce any demonstrable harmful effects. "J. Physical Work. We have demonstrated on the other hand a very marked and significant influence of atmospheric tempera- ture upon the performance of physical work. An increase of room temperature from 68 F. to 75 F. caused a decrease of 15 per cent in the physical work performed by men who were not compelled to maximum effort but were stimulated by a cash bonus ; and an increase from 68 F. to 86 F. with 80 per cent relative humidity caused a decrease of 28 per cent in the physical work performed under conditions of maximal effort:. The fall at 75 F. was most marked in the afternoon hours when fatigue effects were called into play. "F. Susceptibility to Disease. Finally, we have found very definite evidence of the harmful influence of moderately high atmospheric temperatures, particularly if followed by sudden exposure to low temperatures, in promoting susceptibility to bacterial infection. We find that rabbits maintained at a tem- perature of 86 F. show a distinctly delayed formation of hemo- lysins and a slightly reduced agglutinative power, as compared with animals kept at 68 F. ; and that rabbits kept at 76- 70 F. and then chilled to 20-50 or chilled first and kept at 79 F. are much more susceptible to infection than animals kept at 65-70 F. "Exhaustive observations of the nasal mucosa of human sub- jects show that in a warm atmosphere there is an increase, in a cool atmosphere a decrease in the swelling, moisture, and red- ness of the nasal mucous membranes. Sudden change from a hot HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 169 to a cold atmosphere, particularly when combined with drafts, produces a moist and distended but anemic condition of the mucosa, presumably highly favorable to bacterial invasion ; and workers who have been habitually exposed to high temperatures show a marked excess of atrophic rhinitis. Hot moist air (as in the case of laundry workers) seems to be much more harmful than hot dry air (as in the case of furnace men). "//. The Effect of Chemically Vitiated Air upon Physiological State, upon Comfort, and upon Efficiency "In parallel with our experiments upon the effect of tempera- ture, we also studied the possible influence of air, chemically vitiated by human occupancy, but at the same temperature and humidity as fresh air used as a control. The factor of air move- ment was excluded by the use of room fans to stir up both fresh and stale air so that no difference existed save the very slight changes in oxygen and carbon dioxide and the more obvious changes in odoriferous organic constituents due to respiration and effluvia from the body. "A. Physiological Reactions, Comfort, Mental Efficiency, and Resistance to Infection. In most of the reactions studied in our experiments the influence of chemical vitiation of the atmos- phere appeared to be absolutely nil. Temperature and humidity being the same, we compared fresh air containing 5 to 11 parts per 10,000 of carbon dioxide with vitiated air containing 23 to 66 parts and found no difference in body temperature, heart rate, blood pressure, Crampton index, rate of respiration, dead space in the lungs, acidosis of the blood, respiratory quotient, rate of heat production, rate of digestion, and protein metabo- lism. "Comfort votes indicated that the subjects were quite unable to distinguish from the standpoint of sensation between the fresh and the stale air conditions ; the performance of mental work was quite unaffected by the chemically vitiated atmosphere. 170 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE "In a special series of animal experiments, guinea pigs ex- posed to strong fecal odors for considerable periods failed to exhibit any increased susceptibility to inoculations with foreign bacteria or to injection of diphtheria toxin. "B. Physical Work. In regard to the performance of physical work on the other hand, there appeared to be a distinctly harm- ful influence of the vitiated air. Temperature and humidity being the same, our subjects performed 9 per cent less work in stale than in fresh air, a difference less marked than that produced by warm as compared with cool air (15 per cent) but apparently significant. When both unfavorable conditions were combined (in warm and stale air) only 77 per cent as much physical work was performed as in cool fresh air. "C. Appetite and Growth. Finally we found a marked influence exerted by stale air upon the appetite for food as determined by serving standard lunches to parallel groups of subjects in stale and fresh air, respectively, but with the same temperature and humidity. In the four different scries of experiments which were successfully completed on this basis without the intrusion of in- terfering factors, the excess of food consumed under fresh air conditions was respectively 4.4, 6.8, 8.6, and 13.6 per cent. Since the probable errors involved in these experiments were relatively very slight it seems evident that the chemical constituents of vitiated air may not only diminish the tendency to do physical work but also the appetite for food. "This conclusion is strengthened from another direction by demonstration that exposure to strong fecal odors causes a restraining influence upon the rate of growth of guinea pigs during, but not after, the first week of exposure. '7/7. Practical Conclusions in Regard to Ideal Conditions of Ventilation "The experiments of the commission have in general con- firmed the conclusion of earlier investigators that the first and HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 171 foremost condition to be avoided in regulating the atmosphere of occupied rooms is an excessively high temperature. We have found that even slight overheating (75 F.) produces the follow- ing harmful results : "(1) A burden upon the heat-regulating system of the body leading to an increased body temperature, an increased heart rate and a marked decrease in general vaso-motor tone as regis- tered by a fall in the Crampton index. "(2) A slight but definite increase in rate of respiration. "(3) A considerable decrease in the amount of physical work performed under conditions of equal incentive a decrease amounting to 15 per cent at 75 F. and to 28 per cent at 86 F. "(4) A markedly abnormal reaction of the mucous mem- branes of the nose, leading ultimately to chronic atrophic rhini- tis and when followed by chill, producing a moist and distended condition of the membranes calculated to favor bacterial inva- sion. In animals exposure to high atmospheric temperatures, particularly when followed by chill, diminishes the protective power of the blood and markedly increases general susceptibility to microbic disease. "For these reasons we believe that the dangers of room over- heating are far more serious in their effect upon human health and efficiency than has generally been realized and that every effort should be made to keep the temperature of the school- room, the workroom, and the living-room at 68 F. or below. "With regard to the problem of relative humidity it is obvious that a high moisture content combined with high temperature must always be harmful, since the effect of a humid atmosphere is to decrease the heat loss from the body by evaporation. The specifically harmful influence of unduly low humidity which has been postulated by various writers upon ventilation has, on the other hand, not been apparent in our investigations. "Our results in regard to the influence of the chemical com- posHion of vitiated air (temperature and humidity effects being 172 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE excluded) have been generally negative. In two respects, how- ever, our experiments suggest that some chemical constituents of the air of an unventilated room may be objectionable. Such air appears (1) to decrease the appetite of human subjects for food, and (2) to diminish substantially the amount of physical work performed under conditions of equivalent stimulation. "We may conclude then that the primary condition of good ventilation is the maintenance of a room temperature of 68 F. or below without the production of chilling drafts ; but that it is also important, on account of certain subtle but real effects of vitiated air upon appetite and inclination to work, to pro- vide for an air change sufficient to avoid a heavy concentration of effluvia such as was associated in our experiments with a car- bon dioxide content of 23 to 66 parts per 10,000." Except in respect to humidity at moderate and low tempera- tures this long quotation reenforces the conclusions set forth in this book and in World Power and Evolution. It brings out the extreme sensitiveness of human health to atmospheric condi- tions; it shows that temperature is undoubtedly the most im- portant factor; it adds experimental proof to the widespread opinion that as soon as the temperature rises much above 70, man's capacity and inclination for physical work decline and his susceptibility to disease increases. If high atmospheric hu- midity is added to high temperature, as in many tropical coun- tries, the harmful effects are shown to be much accentuated. The fact that the Ventilation Commission detected no effect of high temperature and humidity upon mental work seems to mean merely that when people are subjected to moderately adverse conditions for short periods amounting to less than twenty per cent of the time for a few weeks the physical handicaps do not become pronounced enough to exert a measureable influence upon the mind. It seems only logical to suppose that if the ad- verse physical conditions arising from high temperature and HEALTH AND THE ATMOSPHERE 173 high humidity were to continue and produce their full effect, the mental powers would ultimately suffer. Thus even this phase of the Commission's work is not inconsistent with the conclusions of this book. In this same connection it is interesting to note that although the Commission made no experiments on the effect of variability, its report contains in several places the specific sug- gestion that variability may be an important but unconsidered factor. The only point wherein the Commission's conclusions radically differ from my own is in respect to humidity, a subject which we shall consider in the next chapter. In spite of this dis- crepancy the report of the Ventilation Commission, in its main aspects, confirms the general conclusions of this book as to the relatively depressing effects of tropical climates, regardless of specific diseases ; and as to the beneficial effects of relatively cool, variable climates. CHAPTER VIII MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY INASMUCH as there is some disagreement as to the effect of atmospheric moisture upon health, it will be well to examine the evidence carefully. On the basis of the deaths in Paris from 1904 to 1913, Besson,* the chief of the "Service Physique et Meteorologique" of that city, has come to the conclusion that "on the whole, when the humidity increases, the mortality [from diseases of the respiratory organs] decreases two or three weeks later. ... If one examines each season separately one finds that this rule fails only in summer when there is no clear result, According to a widespread opinion humidity acts in an un- favorable manner upon human health. The result announced above cannot fail to surprise many people. One sees that an in- crease in the number of deaths from diseases of the respiratory organs is on an average preceded not by an increase but by a decrease in the humidity." To test this further, Besson divided the winter months into two groups, one with a mean relative humidity below 86 per cent and averaging 82.4 and the other above 86 per cent and averaging 89.2. He found that the deaths per week when relatively high or low humidity prevailed and in the succeeeding weeks averaged as follows : Advantage of Deaths per week high over low Low High humidity in per humidity humidity cent of low The same week 163 161 1.2% One week after 177 167 6.0% Two weeks after 185 175 5.7% Three weeks after 191 183 4.4% Four weeks after 195 196 0.5% * Louis Besson: Relations Entre les Elements M6t6orologiques et la Mortalite. Annales des Services Techniques d'Hygiene de la Ville de Paris, 1921. MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 175 It seems clear that during these ten years in Paris the drier weeks of winter, even though they were quite moist, were accom- panied by a slightly increased death rate from respiratory dis- eases and were followed in the next two weeks by a death rate about 6 per cent higher than that which followed the moist weeks. Besson ascribes part of this effect to the direction of the wind, but the effect of the wind must be produced largely through humidity and temperature. What part is played by temperature in the results for Paris is not clear. A study of my own includes temperature as well as humidity, and gives results almost identical with those of Besson. I used the deaths from pneumonia in New York City during the year beginning in April, 1917, and compared them with the weather on the day of death and on the preceding day. All the days with any given temperature were divided into two equal groups on the basis of their relative humidit}'. Here are the results for the 7200 deaths from lobar pneumonia. Those for broncho-pneu- monia were somewhat similar, but much more irregular, pre- sumably because the number of deaths was only a third as great. AVERAGE DEATHS PER DAY FROM LOBAR PNEUMONIA ON SAME DAY AS GIVEN WEATHER CONDITIONS AND ON SUCCEEDING DAY. (NEW YORK, APRIL 1, 1917, TO MARCH 31, 1918.) Mean temperature 20 or less 21-32 38-45 46-55 56-65 G6-70 71-75 76 or over Low relative humidity 28.4 26.7 28.7 21.5 19.8 10.5 6.5 6.8 High relative humidity 26.3 25.9 28.0 18.8 15.1 8.2 6.1 6.5 Advantage of moist over dry 8.0% 3.1% 2.5% 14.3% 31.1% 28.0% 6.6% 4.4% The obvious fact here is that at all temperatures the moist days were better than the dry, as appears in the lower line. The same thing holds true whether we consider the deaths on the 176 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE same day as the humidity or on the succeeding day. While Bes- son's data seem to show that dryness renders people susceptible to the initial attack of pneumonia which results in death after a fortnight more or less, the New York data suggest that when the disease nears its crisis a dry day may turn the balance toward death, whereas a moist day turns it toward life. In cold weather, however, this effect is slight, for when the temperature is below 45 the moist days in the preceding table reduce the death rate only 4.5 per cent on an average. At the temperatures of 56 to 70, on the other hand, an additional grain and a half of water vapor per cubic foot of space, or a difference of roughly 20 per cent in relative humidity, is associated with a diminution of about 30 per cent in the death rate. This is especially im- portant because these are the temperatures at which our houses are kept, or ought to be kept, most of the year. It adds another to the bits of evidence which indicate that for respiratory dis- eases a dry climate is worse than a moist one. The opposite belief has perhaps become traditional largely because in dry climates people live out of doors. Other things being equal, it is always more healthful to live outdoors rather than indoors. In this connection it might be added that Besson's conclusions as to the relation of temperature to the death rate from respira- tory diseases agree with mine as to the similar relation to deaths from each of the two main types of pneumonia. At temperatures between freezing and 60 F. there is an almost perfectly regular decline in the number of deaths as the temperature rises ; then the decline becomes less and less marked until a minimum is reached at about 72. At higher temperatures a slight increase makes itself apparent, but does not go far because there are only a few days in either Paris or New York when the mean temperature rises much above 75. Still another investigation, that of Greenburg,* who studied the monthly deaths from pneu- * David Greenburg: Relation of Meteorological Conditions to the Preva- lence of Pneumonia. Journal of American Medical Association, 1919, p. 252. MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 177 mo ilia in New York, Boston, Newark, and Providence, agrees with the two already cited as to the effect of both temperature and humidity upon pneumonia. Such close agreement makes it practically certain that the humidity of the air, as well as the temperature, is an important element in determining the death rate from respiratory diseases. The need of certainty as to the effect of atmospheric humidity is so great that I shall sum up a number of other examples in the form of a long table which the non-scientific reader can skip. Since the effect of humidity varies according to temperature, we must carefully distinguish between temperatures above and below the optimum of 64. At the optimum temperature in every one of the groups of deaths listed in section A of this table, the best conditions of health prevailed when the relative humidity averaged 70 per cent or more for day and night together. At higher temperatures a relative humidity which averages above 70 per cent (for day and night together) does very decided harm. At the optimum temperature the effect of humidity seems to be at a minimum, and a humidity above 70 per cent does little harm. A lower humidity, although somewhat harmful, has less effect than at other temperatures, the increase in the death rate ranging from 5 to 15 per cent, according to the degree of dry- ness. This may be one reason why the New York Ventilation Commission found no clear effect of low humidity. Their main experiments were performed at temperatures close to the opti- mum. At temperatures below the optimum the effect of humidity upon the death rate is very clear, much more so than at the opti- mum. For example, at a temperature of about 40 F. a difference of only 10 per cent in humidity appears to produce approxi- mately the effect shown in section A of the following table. 178 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE EFFECT OF HUMIDITY ON THE DEATH RATE A. Increase in Monthly Death Rate Accompanying a Decrease of 10 Per Cent in Relative Humidity at a Temperature of 40 F. (1) Northeastern United States, 1900-1912 0.8% (2) East central United States, 1900-1912 1.3% (3) Dry interior of the United States, 1900-1912 2.0% (4) Boston (deaths after operations), 1906-1915 3.1% (5) Large cities of United States (whites non-contagious dis- eases), 1912-1915 3.3% (6) Large cities of United States (negroes, non-contagious dis- eases), 1912-1915 4.0% (7) British Columbia, 1914-1916 7.0% (8) Large cities of United States (whites, contagious diseases), 1912-1915 9.5% (9) Southern France, 1901-1910 10.0% (10) Northern France, 1901-1910 11.0% (11) Northern Italy, 1899-1913 12.6% B. Increase in Monthly Death Rate Accompanying a Decrease of 10 Per Cent in Relative Humidity at All Temperatures, December-March, 1900-1914 (12) St. Louis 0.5% (13) New York City 1.2% (14) San Francisco 3.8% (15) Baltimore 5.0% (16) Chicago 7.8% C. Increase in Daily Death Rate Accompanying a Decrease of 10 Per Cent in Relative Humidity at All Temperatures During the Influenza Epidemic in New York City (September-December, 1918) and Boston (October, 1918-April, 1919) NOTE. The figures in this section indicate percentages of the average daily change in the death rate instead of percentages of the actual deaths. Hence the figures are perhaps five times larger than if they were reckoned as in A and B. (17) Onset of influenza, Boston 3.0% (18) Onset of influenza, New York 6.9% (19) Deaths from influenza, Boston 11.3% (20) Deaths from pneumonia, New York 21.6% (21) Deaths from influenza, New York 26.2% (22) Deaths from pneumonia, Boston 8.3% (23) Onset of pneumonia, Boston 1.8% MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 179 This table represents all the available mortality data except those already mentioned and certain others to be given shortly. Practically all the evidence seems to point in the same direction. In 21 out of the 23 sets of data in the table the moister days or months have an advantage over the drier. The negative figures for Boston, Nos. 22 and 23, may be accidental, or may mean that Boston's famous east winds, unlike the moist winds in most places, are really too damp. Taking the table as a whole, an increase of 10 per cent in humidity at low temperatures is corre- lated with an average decrease of not far from 6 per cent in the death rate. Let us now turn to quite a different investigation. In Boston I made a study of the number of deaths following operations performed in different kinds of weather. On the basis of about 2300 deaths after operations from 1906-1915 the addition of a grain of moisture per cubic foot of space to the air within doors would have diminished the death rate as follows, provided the inside air thereby acquired the qualities pertaining to the outside air moistened by nature : APPARENT EFFECT OF ONE GRAIN OF WATER PER CUBIC FOOT OF AIR IN DIMINISHING THE DEATH RATE AFTER OPERATIONS IN BOSTON, 1906-1915 Percentage of decrease in deaths for one additional A. Mean temperature grain of water per cubic foot 30 or less 9.2% 31-40 . . . . 16.5% 41-50 8.8% 50-60 .... . . 13.7% 61-70 4.6% Because of the small number of deaths this table is irregular, but the irregularities have little significance. At temperatures below the optimum the death rate was higher after operations 180 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE performed in damp weather than after those performed when the air was dry. This was especially true when the moist weather continued a day or two after the operations. At high tempera- tures, however, the effect of humidity is not at all the same as at low, as appears in Figure 15. Dry conditions are shown toward 100 >0.75 I C.50 I & 0.25 a> Relative Humidiiy at 6 AH K> 60 SO Above 70'al 6 AM. 4}-SO'at 8 AM 61-70*ctt 8AM 51-60'ai 8AM Figure 15. Post-operative Death Rate at Boston in Relation to Humidity and Temperature the left, moist toward the right. The height of the lines shows the number of deaths per day succeeding operations performed when various conditions of relative humidity prevailed at 8 a.m. The broken lines, A, B, and C, indicate the number of deaths in cool weather. Note their regular decline toward the right. High humidity was evidently an advantage. Now contrast the dotted lines with the solid line, A, indicating the effect of humidity when the temperature at 8 a.m. was above 70. Under such con- ditions, far more than in cooler weather, dry air was bad. Moderately moist air with a relative humidity of 50 to 60 per cent was better than even the moistest air at lower temperatures. MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 181 But note how rapidly the death rate after operations performed in hot weather rises if high humidity is added to great heat. Nevertheless the evil effects of very damp hot days in Boston do not appear to be so bad as those of very dry hot days. This study of surgical operations seems to afford strong evidence of the extreme sensitiveness of the human body to variations in hu- midity as well as temperature. The fact that it gives a different result at temperatures above and below the optimum tends to establish confidence, for the results at high temperature and high humidity are in perfect accord with common experience and with the experiments carried on by such organizations as the New York Ventilation Commission and the laboratory of the American Society of Heating and Ventilating Engineers at Pittsburgh. In other countries as well as in France, Italy, and the United States, whence our data have thus far come, the statistics seem usually to indicate that extreme dryness is harmful to health. Other things being equal, the death rates appear generally to be higher in dry regions than in moist. For example, Lucknow and Cairo in dry climates have death rates far higher than Madras and Bombay, which are moist and tropical. Mexico City on its high, cool, but dry plateau, and Johannesburg and Madrid in somewhat similar locations, have exceptionally high death rates in view of their temperature and latitude. In Mexico and India the dry season of March, April, and May has a decidedly higher death rate than the succeeding wet season. This happens in spite of the fact that the temperature in Mexico City during May averages 65 and is almost ideal, while in July it averages a degree or two cooler and is almost equally ideal. In India, on the other hand, both the dry spring and the wet summer are hot, so that the wet season is very muggy. Yet the people heave a sigh of relief when the rains come, for it brings hope of a diminution of disease as well as of good crops. Let us turn to variability or storminess, another factor which 182 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE seems to cooperate with temperature and humidity in determin- ing the effect of climate. This factor is especially important be- cause of its bearing on changes of climate and their effect on history. In World Power and Evolution the study of climo- graphs based on eight million deaths in France, Italy, and the United States suggests that people's sensitiveness to changes of temperature is almost directly proportional to the uniformity of their climate. For example, in San Francisco a change of only 7 in the mean monthly temperature (50 January, 57 July) is associated with a change in the death rate from I6A per cent above normal to 11.7 per cent below, or about 4 per cent for every degree of temperature. At St. Paul and Minne- apolis a change from a mean temperature of 12 in January to 67 in June is accompanied by a change in the death rate from 5.5 per cent above normal to 6.9 below, or only 0.23 per cent for each degree of temperature. In the same way a differ- ence of about 9 C. between January and May in Naples is accompanied by a difference of 35.3 per cent in the death rate, whereas in Milan a difference of 16 C. in temperature is asso- ciated with a difference of only 26.8 per cent in the death rate. Put in another way this means that the apparent effect of a given change of temperature is 2.3 times as great in Naples of the South as in Milan of the North, and over seventeen times as great in San Francisco with its remarkably uniform climate as in St. Paul and Minneapolis with their severe winters and many storms at all seasons. In other words, where great changes of weather take place, people become hardened to them. The reality of this hardening is demonstrated again and again by the way in which north- erners who move to the tropics lose their power of resistance to even the slightest changes of temperature, but regain it after a few years of renewed residence in a severe climate. The relation between variability and the power to resist disease seems quite clear, but as yet there seems to be no conclusive evidence as to MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 183 the relative importance of variations from day to day such as accompany storms, and of variations from season to season. The evidence which will now be set forth pertains to changes of temperature from day to day. In World Power and Evolution one of the most important lines of evidence has to do with variations of temperature from one day to the next in New York City. A study of about 400,000 deaths during a period of eight years from 1877 to 1884 shows that when the temperature falls the death rate also falls, while a rise in temperature is regularly accompanied by a correspond- ing rise in the death rate. This happens not only in summer, when one would expect it, but at all seasons, including even the winter, when one would surely suppose that warm weather would be beneficial. And so it is, in the long run, but the immediate change toward warmth is temporarily harmful. In this investigation, instead of employing the absolute num- ber of deaths, as in previous cases, the change in the number of deaths from one day to the next has been used. This is done partly in order that the greater frequency of days with abrupt changes of temperature in winter than in summer may not con- fuse our results. It is likewise because in dealing with changes of temperature the natural question is whether such changes have any effect in changing the death rate. The upper part of Figure 16 illustrates another investigation of the same kind in respect to daily deaths from pneumonia in New York. The left-hand side of the diagram indicates the con- ditions which prevail when the mean temperature of the day of death is higher than that of the preceding day, while the right- hand side indicates that the temperature has fallen. The two solid lines indicate the summer relationships from April to Sep- tember, A being lobar pneumonia, and B broncho-pneumonia. The dotted lines show winter conditions, C lobar, and D broncho- pneumonia. The significant points about these four curves are as follows : 184 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE (1) They are all essentially alike. This suggests that they all conform to some definite physiological law. (2) Every one of them is low at the two ends and high in the Rise of Temperature Drop of Temperature 10* 5' 5" JO'F 2.0 -ZCfc -1.0 -zo 205 Deaths from Pneumonia. In New York. Apr 1917 lo Mar 1918 Deaths from Pneunronia and Inflaenza, com- bined during Epidemic of 1918 New York - -Boston Figure 16. Relation between Deaths from Pneumonia and Influenza and Interdiurnal Changes of Temperature middle. This seems to indicate that under normal conditions such as prevailed in the year ending in March, 1918, patients suffer- ing from either form of pneumonia are less likely to die on days when there has been a marked change of temperature in either MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 185 direction than on days when there is little or no change. This conclusion applies to both summer and winter, but is more true in summer than winter. Hence, for pneumonia patients, a vari- able climate seems better than one that is uniform. The agree- ment of the curves with our conclusions derived from factory work and with those of Campani in Italy is noteworthy. (3) A drop of temperature, on an average, is decidedly more effective than a rise in lowering the death rate. This again agrees with the results of our factory investigation. The lower part of Figure 16 shows the results of a similar investigation of the influenza epidemic in the latter part of 1918. Because of the enormous variations in the death rate I have here used the percentage rather than the actual number of deaths by which each day's death toll differed from that of the pre- ceding day. Both New York (solid lines) and Boston (dotted lines) have been included. The investigation has been broadened to include (1) the number of cases in which influenza (a) and pneumonia (b) attacked a patient, or at least, in the case of Boston, were reported to have done so, and (2) the number of deaths from influenza (c) and pneumonia (d). The significant points in Figure 16 are as follows : (1) All the lines except that for deaths from pneumonia in Boston (c') slope in the same way, thus indicating that a rise of temperature is worse than no change, and that a fall is much better than either. This disagrees with the preceding investiga- tion of normal pneumonia, but that may be simply because the lower part of Figure 16 includes only the winter months and not those of the spring when the advent of warm days would be beneficial. (2) The average changes in the rates for all the conditions shown in the lower part of Figure 16 amount to an increase of 14.1 per cent in the deaths and illnesses on days with a strong rise of temperature and 3.4 per cent on days with little or no change, whereas on days with a strong drop of temperature the 186 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE change takes the form of a drop of 18.4 per cent. This means that for all the days when the temperature was markedly dif- ferent from that of the preceding days, no matter whether it was higher or lower, there was an average drop of 2.2 per cent in the rates of disease and death against a rise of 3.4 per cent on the days of comparatively uniform weather. In other words, here, just as in normal pneumonia, the variable weather had an ad- vantage over the uniform weather. This conclusion is quite con- trary to the usual ideas, but it is supported by many other bits of evidence. One such bit is found in the following data as to the average number of deaths per day in Boston hospitals after surgical operations, when specified changes of temperature took place between the day of an operation and the succeeding day. Interdiurnal change of temperature Daily deaths December to February Daily deaths March to November Changes of 9 F. or more in either direction Change of 4 to 8 in either direction . Change of 3 or less in either direction . 0.340 0.336 0.326 0.235 0.288 0.343 The first column suggests that in winter the changes of tem- perature are too severe in Boston, so that days with only a little change have an advantage of about 4 per cent over the d&ys with a violent change. From March to November, however, the conditions are quite different: operations performed on days with the smallest changes are followed by 41 per cent more deaths than are those performed when the temperature changed most strongly from the day of the operation to the next day. Thus for the year as a whole the variable weather displays a distinct advantage just as in New York. The net result of our studies thus far is the conclusion that temperature, humidity, and variability all play important parts MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 187 in determining variations in health and mortality from day to day, month to month, season to season, and year to year. For each climatic factor there appears to be a distinct optimum or most favorable condition, but this varies considerably in re- sponse to differences in the other factors. Thus the optimum temperature in dry weather is not the same as in wet, and pre- sumably is not the same in variable weather as in that which is uniform. The high level of the optimum temperature which we found in California may possibly be due in part to the uniformity of the weather. Again, a degree of humidity which is highly favorable at a temperature of 50 F. may work severe harm at 80, just as a degree of variability which is highly beneficial in warm weather may be too extreme in the midst of a cold winter. In the rest of this chapter I shall describe certain investiga- tions in which the three climatic factors of temperature, hu- midity, and variability are analyzed in reference to the influenza epidemic in 1918. The investigation of influenza was carried out by a Committee on the Atmosphere and Man appointed by the National Research Council of the United States. A mathe- matical method known as partial correlation coefficients was em- ployed. This method has the remarkable quality of picking out and isolating the effect of any one among a number of factors as in an experiment. In the present case the Committee set itself the completion, or at least the extension, of a task already begun by Professor Pearl of Johns Hopkins University. The task was to ascertain whether any environmental conditions were re- sponsible for the great differences in the mortality of the influ- enza epidemic of 1918 from one city to another. During the ten weeks of the main epidemic Philadelphia, for example, had a death rate from influenza and the resultant pneumonia four times as great as that of Milwaukee, while the rate in Pittsburgh was twice as great as in the neighboring and similar city of Cleve- land. Before the committee's investigation was finished the fol- lowing twenty-two factors had been examined : 188 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE A. Factors of human environment (demography). 1. Age (proportion of inhabitants of various ages). 2. Sex (number of females per hundred males). 3. Density of the population (persons per acre within city). 4. Rate of growth from 1900 to 1910. B. Factors of geographical position. 5. Distance from Boston, where the epidemic began. 6. Longitude. 7. Latitude. C. Physiological factors; normal death rates in 1916, 1916, and 1917 from: 8. All causes. 9. Pulmonary tuberculosis. 10. Organic diseases of the heart. 11. Nephritis and acute B right's disease. 12. Typhoid fever. 13. Cancer and other malignant tumors. D. Racial factors. 14. Percentage of negroes, 1920. 15. Percentage of foreign born, 1920 E. Industrial factor. 16. Percentage of population engaged in manufacturing, 1919. F. Climatic factors. 17. Mean temperature for day and night. 18. Change of mean temperature from one day to the next. 19. Absolute humidity (weight of water vapor per cubic foot of space). 20. Relative humidity, or percentage of possible water vapor. 21. Weather a combination of Nos. 17-20. 22. Climatic energy as denned in this book. Directly or indirectly these twenty-two factors embrace most of the conditions which may have been effective in causing people's power of resistance to the epidemic to vary from city to city. Sanitation and medical practice fail to appear in the list because their degree of excellence cannot easily be expressed in figures. But the death rate from typhoid fever is generally sup- posed to be an unusually good measure of sanitary efficiency, while other death rates are in most places a fairly good index of the excellence of the medical service. Almost the only im- MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 189 portant field which the factors do not cover is that of variations in the disease-bringing bacteria so far as such variations are due to causes not included in our table. When all these various factors are investigated by means of the most exact and delicate mathematical method yet known, the only one which shows any conclusive causal relation to the destructiveness of this particu- lar epidemic is the weather. In the work which ultimately led to this conclusion, the Com- mittee on the Atmosphere and Man took the death rate from influenza and pneumonia during the ten weeks succeeding the outbreak of the epidemic in each of thirty-six large cities in the United States. These ten weeks cover the first and, in most places, much the more important outbreak. The committee also obtained data as to the temperature, relative humidity, absolute humidity, and change of temperature from one day to the next. The weather data were tabulated for periods of ten days be- ginning seventy days before the onset of the epidemic and con- tinuing fifty days thereafter. Previous to the thirtieth day before the epidemic there is evidence of no real relationship be- tween any weather condition and the destructiveness of the in- fluenza. During the thirty days just before the onset of the epidemic, however, the temperature and especially the absolute, as distinguished from the relative humidity show a distinct rela- tion to the succeeding death rate. This means that if the weather was warm during the month before the influenza reached a city, the death rate was high; if the amount of moisture in the air was great, the conditions were still worse. At Boston, for ex- ample, from the twentieth to the eleventh day before the epi- demic the temperature was higher than during the corresponding period in any other cities except New Orleans, New York, and Los Angeles. This was natural, for the epidemic broke out in Bos- ton earlier than elsewhere. In places like St. Paul, Toledo, and Grand Rapids, the cool and fairly dry autumn weather which prevailed for a month before the epidemic apparently gave peo- 190 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE pie a certain degree of stored-up vigor which stood them in good stead and lessened the ravages of the disease. If the tempera- ture was variable, as in Cleveland, Columbus, and Richmond, and especially if it fell during the ten days after the onset of the epidemic, the death rate was lower than where the contrary conditions prevailed. On the other hand, high relative humidity during the ten days before the onset was associated with a relatively high death rate. Cambridge, New Haven, and New Orleans suffered most in this respect. The dampness perhaps made it easy for the bacteria to be transmitted. Droplets of water in the air may act as carriers of the bacteria, or may pre- serve their virility. From the tenth to the thirtieth days after the onset of the epidemic the virulence of the bacteria was apparently so great that the state of the weather made no difference in the death rate. At any rate there is no evidence that the immediate weather conditions had any effect in overcoming the sudden and sweeping character of the infection. After the thirtieth day, however, there came another change, and the apparent effects of tempera- ture and absolute humidity again rose high. This was the time when in most places the disease reached its maximum and began to decline. At that time cool and moderately dry weather once more was associated with a low death rate. This does not neces- sarily mean that cold weather is favorable at the time of an epidemic. In fact, quite the contrary may be the case, for very low temperature may be as bad as high. Labrador suffered greatly in the epidemic of 1918. Having reached the conclusion that atmospheric conditions influenced the severity of the epidemic, the next step was to find a numerical expression for the weather by combining the tem- perature, humidity, and variability according to their apparent importance. When this had been done, the method of partial correlations was used to compare the weather with the severity of the epidemic and with all the other factors which showed any MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 191 sign of being important, namely, deaths from tuberculosis, deaths from all causes, deaths from heart diseases, and climatic energy. The weather proved to be the only one whose correlation coefficient was more than four times the probable error, and hence large enough to be significant. The final partial correla- tion coefficient, when the four other factors named above were held constant, that is eliminated, amounts to 0.57. This is 7.6 times the probable error, which means that there is only one chance in hundreds of millions that we are being misled by acci- dental agreements between the weather and the death rate. "Thus," to quote the report of the committee, "the statistical fact is clear. The weather, which means primarily the weather just before the onset of the epidemic and at or just after the climax, is the one factor thus far investigated which shows a clear, pronounced, and persistent relation to the destructiveness of the epidemic. This does not mean that the weather was in any sense a cause of the epidemic. It is even possible that the weather may be related to the epidemic only indirectly, as is the case with the death rate from heart disease, although no factor capable of producing this result has yet been suggested. Even if the weather is a causal factor in producing variations in the virulence of the epidemic, there is no reason to think that it is the only factor. If the degree of relationship between two vari- ables is proportional to the square of the correlation coefficient, as is sometimes held, the weather, even if it is a direct a;^ent, may be responsible for no more than a third of the variations from city to city. Nor do our high correlation coefficients mean that the weather had anything to do with setting the date of the epi- demic or with determining the severity of the 1918 epidemic compared with other epidemics. Neither do they prove anything as to the effect of the weather in other countries, although else- where a relationship similar to that found in the United States seems probable. For instance, the British government estimates that in India the death rate from the epidemic was about six 192 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE times as great as in the United States, while scanty reports from other tropical countries indicate a similar excessive mortality. The one thing which seems clear from the present investigation is that the weather is the one factor whose apparent relation- ship to the epidemic is not seriously reduced or modified when other conditions are held constant. "The results of this investigation should be qualified in still another respect. It is not necessary to suppose that other epi- demics will show exactly the same relationships as the epidemic of 1918, even though they may be strongly influenced by the weather. In the first place, the epidemic of 1918 was so peculiar in its virulence, its rapid dissemination, its fatality for persons in the prime of life, and in other respects, that it may well have been peculiar in its climatic relationships. In the second place, the epidemic occurred at a season when the approach of cold weather normally exerts a strong stimulating effect in the United States. It is well known that from August to October or even November the death rate normally declines. The epidemic seems to have reflected this condition. Just so far as the weather approached the conditions which prevail at the time when the autumn mortality is lowest, the ravages of the epidemic were checked. At some other, colder season, relatively low tempera- ture and low humidity might be as harmful as high temperature and high humidity appear to have been in September, October, and November, 1918. As a matter of fact, the epidemic of Feb- ruary and March, 1919, shows only a small positive correlation between the monthly death rate and the temperature. Other con- ditions, perhaps other conditions of weather, were then domi- nant; or possibly some cities were too cold while others were too warm, a condition which would make the use of correlation coefficients impracticable. "Finally, even if the weather should prove to be an important factor in causing variations in the virulence of influenza, we still have little evidence as to how its effects are produced. Presum- MORTALITY, MOISTURE, AND VARIABILITY 193 ably the weather gives to the human being more or less power of resistance to disease. But it is not improbable that the weather also has an important effect upon the vigor, reproductive rate, or transmission of the disease-bringing bacteria." CHAPTER IX HEALTH AND WEATHER THIS chapter deals with two investigations of the relation between health and the weather. They seem to me the most conclusive evidence yet available along this line because the various weather elements are more clearly separated than in most cases, and because there is no danger of confusing the effects of different seasons. The first investigation pertains to the weather day by day, and is by far the most extensive in which daily data have been employed. It represents a coopera- tive effort carried on by the Committee on the Atmosphere and Man of the National Research Council of the United States and the Metropolitan and New York Life Insurance companies. In addition to the author those most closely concerned in planning the work were Dr. J. Arthur Harris of the Carnegie Institution's laboratory at Cold Spring Harbor and Dr. L. I. Dublin of the Metropolitan Life Insurance Company, but many suggestions were received from others. The results appear to be fairly con- clusive as to mean temperature and changes of temperature, but are inconclusive as to relative humidity. In order to obtain an adequate series of daily mortality data the Committee was obliged to go back to the years 1882 to 1888 in New York City. At that time and for a few years previous the actual day of death was recorded and the facts were sum- marized by days and published in the annual reports of the New York Board of Health. This highly valuable record has not since been equalled either in New York or elsewhere, so far as I am aware. The Committee used three sets of mortality data: HEALTH AND WEATHER 195 (1) deaths of children under five years of age; (2) deaths of persons over five years of age; (3) deaths from pneumonia. In order to avoid errors due to the growth of population and the improvement in medical practice, each year was treated as a separate unit, and each category of deaths was reduced to per- centages of the average number of deaths per day in that par- ticular year. For the phase of the investigation here under discussion, the method of partial correlation coefficients was employed. For the layman it may be well to repeat that by this method the effects of different factors can be separated as in an experiment. In order to avoid periods when the temperature is sometimes above and sometimes below the optimum, the months of De- cember to March were chosen. This is also advisable because the effect of the various climatic elements in winter is less under- stood than their effect in summer. One aim of the work was to eliminate the effect of the seasons and determine whether a mere departure of the weather elements from the normal for any special month has any effect. Accordingly, in preparing the data for the correlation coefficients each month was treated as a sepa- rate unit, and the departures of all kinds were reckoned from the averages for December, January, and so forth. This made it possible to ascertain almost beyond question that the weather day by day causes small variations in health which are super- posed upon the large seasonal variations. The Committee used three elements of the weather: (1) the mean daily temperature; (2) the interdiurnal change of mean temperature from one day to the next; and (3) the mean daily relative humidity. In nature the effects of these three elements are inextricably mixed, but the method of partial correlation sorts them out. The results of this sorting appear in Figure 17. The day marked zero is the day on which a given condition of weather occurs. The weather elements on each such day have been compared with the deaths on that day and on each of the Mean Temp&raiurc l,,k r-diun?al Change of Cha/jyes of Tempemlure iu..{ TciY)peTCllUTe Rdalive Hiunidity hdil totwl^ Temp and Retatwe uniid% Comtant Relative Humidity Temperature and Changes of Temperature hriUl Ccmalank C e Fx B odhs over ve Ycur5 G R, f UJb -.15 : 17. Correlation between Weather Elements and Daily Deaths in New York City, December to March, 1882-1886 HEALTH AND WEATHER 197 fourteen succeeding days. The lengths of the bars in Figure 17 indicate the size of the partial correlation coefficients. The three diagrams on the left show the relation between mean temperature and the deaths in our three categories when the interdiurnal change of temperature and the relative humidity are held con- stant and thus eliminated. The three central diagrams show similar coefficients for changes of temperature when mean tem- perature and relative humidity are held constant ; while the right- hand set apply to relative humidity when mean temperature and changes of temperature are held constant. Where the bars of Figure 17 lie above the central line, the coefficients are positive ; that is, a high condition of the weather, such as high humidity or high temperature, is associated with a high death rate. Where the bars are below this central line the reverse is the case, high temperature, for example, being asso- ciated with few deaths. But note that in studying changes of temperature our purpose is not to discover the effect of a small change compared with that of a large change regardless of whether the temperature rises or falls. It is to discover whether the effects of a rise and of a fall are the same or different. Ac- cordingly, the average condition has been counted as that in which there is no change of temperature from one day to the next ; a rise of temperature has been given a plus sign and a fall a negative sign. Hence in the central column of Figure 17 a bar above the line means that a high death rate is associated with a rise of temperature, while a bar below the line means that the death rate is high when the temperature falls. The degree of significance of the bars in Figure 17 may be judged from the shading. Where the bars are lightly shaded their length is less than three times the probable error. This means that they have little or no significance. When the light shading reaches its greatest length, that is, when the coefficient is three times the probable error, there is one chance in twenty- two that a bar of this length would be produced accidentally 198 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE even if our two sets of figures have no real relation. Suppose for a moment that the death rate and the relative humidity have no real relationship. Nevertheless, with figures the size of those here used we should accidentally get a correlation of 0.075 (three times the probable error) once in every twenty-two correlations. The three diagrams on the right of Figure 17 depict forty-five correlation coefficients. Hence mere chance would be likely to give us two that rise as high as 0.075. What we actually find is two which rise a little above that level. The areas marked by diagonals indicate values between three and four times the probable error. Now, the likelihood that a coefficient of any particular size will be produced accidentally decreases very rapidly as the coefficients become larger. Thus while there is one chance in twenty-two that a coefficient will be three times the probable error, there is only one chance in one hundred forty-two that it will be four times the probable error. Among the ninety coefficients in the first and second columns of Figure 17 mere chance would not be likely to give more than one coefficient rising to the outer limit of the diagonals, but as a matter of fact we have eleven. If the coefficient is five times the probable error there is only one chance in 1341 that it is due to accident arid not to a real relationship ; if six times, only one in 19,300; seven, 427,000; eight, 14,700,000. For all practical purposes a coefficient eight times the probable error gives full certainty of a relationship of some sort. In general we may say that in Figure 17, or any similar dia- gram, the conditions that suggest a relationship are (1) coeffi- cients more than three times the probable error; (2) a con- siderable series of coefficients all having the same sign and hence all falling either above or below the central line; (3) a series of coefficients which systematically change from high values to low or from positive to negative, or vice versa. The conditions which are generally agreed to amount to practical proof of a relation- ship are ( 1 ) individual coefficients which rise to at least six times HEALTH AND WEATHER 199 the probable error; (2) several successive coefficients rising to at least four times the probable error; (3) a considerable series of coefficients which systematically and persistently change their values in some orderly sequence such as from three or four times the probable error on the plus side to an equal value on the negative side. All three types are represented in Figure 17. We are now ready to interpret that Figure. Bear in mind that the length of the columns indicates the degree of relationship between deaths and the weather elements, and has nothing to do with the actual number of deaths. Remember also that when we speak of temperature in what follows, we mean temperature after the effects of relative humidity and interdiurnal changes of tem- perature have been eliminated by means of partial correlations. In similar fashion relative humidity and changes of temperature mean those two factors individually after the other two have been eliminated. To begin in the upper left-hand corner, the posi- tion of the bar for day above the central line in diagram A indicates that during the 726 days from December to March in the six years under discussion, high temperature on any par- ticular day tended to be accompanied by a large number of deaths of children under five years of age on that same day. The small size of the bar, however, indicates that this relationship is too small to be considered seriously. On the other hand, the temperature on any given day had a pronounced relation to the deaths during the next three days, as appears in the heavy black shading of days 1 to 3. High temperature was systematically followed by a low death rate, and low temperature by a high death rate. Inasmuch as we are dealing with only a single day's weather, and inasmuch as the largest coefficient ( 0.167) is seven times the probable error, the total effect of the mean tem- perature for all days on the deaths among children must be great. On the fourth day after a given temperature, however, it has practically disappeared. Diagram B suggests that the reaction of older persons to the 200 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE outside temperature is similar to that of little children. Curi- ously enough, however, on the day when a given temperature occurs the effect is stronger than among the children, while on succeeding days the opposite effect is weaker and is somewhat more delayed. Probably the immediate harmful effect of high winter temperature arises from the fact that when the outer air is unusually warm for the season, our houses are likely to be kept too warm, especially on the first day of such warmth. On the other hand, when unusually cold days arrive, many houses which have been too warm become cooler and that is helpful, but soon the fires are pushed and the old condition of hot, stuffy rooms returns. In diagram C, showing the relationship between the mean tem- perature and the deaths from pneumonia, the most important feature is the fairly regular decline from a moderately high level on the left to a low level on the right. This apparently means that temperatures which are high for the season tend to cause death among pneumonia patients, but have a good effect in preventing other people from contracting the disease, so that the death rate from pneumonia falls off after about two weeks. Further comment on these first three diagrams is unnecessary. They confirm the results obtained in other ways, and show that the temperature of even a single day plays an appreciable and measurable part in determining the general health of the com- munity. Look now at the middle column of Figure 17. This depicts the relationship between the death rate and the change of tempera- ture from one day to the next when the mean temperature and the relative humidity are both eliminated by means of partial cor- relations. Among children less than five years old, as appears at the top, a rise of temperature tends strongly to cause many deaths on the day when it occurs and on the succeeding day, while a drop acts in the opposite fashion. So strong is this effect that the largest partial correlation coefficient (0.202) is 8.4 HEALTH AND WEATHER 201 times the probable error. Inasmuch as there is scarcely one chance in one hundred million that so large a coefficient should be accidentally obtained, we may be practically certain that changes of temperature from one day to the next (regardless of the mean temperature) exert an important effect upon the health of young children. The suddenness with which this effect comes to an end is noteworthy. The portion of diagram D from the third to the fourteenth day is typical of the coefficients obtained when there is no relationship between two sets of phenomena. Diagram E indicates that among older people changes of temperature from day to day have almost the same effect as among children, a rise being harmful and a drop beneficial. In this case, however, the relationship is not so marked as among the children, the delay is greater, and there is a reaction on the fifth day. Thus the harm done by a rise of temperature, or the good done by a fall, is partly neutralized by effects of the oppo- site kind a few days later, but the neutralization is only partial, as appears from the greater size of the shaded areas above than below the zero line. Pneumonia patients (diagram F) present another case where effects of opposite types occur at different times. People suffer- ing from this disease are probably harmed somewhat by a rise of temperature on the very day when it happens, and are simi- larly helped by a fall, but these effects are too slight to be sig- nificant. Two days after a given change of temperature, on the contrary, the pneumonia patients show a distinct benefit if the change has been toward warmer conditions ; they are harmed by a change in the opposite direction. This occurs regardless of whether the actual mean temperature is high or low, for that factor has been eliminated by our partial correlations, as has relative humidity. The changes themselves appear to be the effective agent. But how about the relatively high positive corre- lation on the eleventh day in diagram F? There is about one chance in one hundred and fifty that this is due to accident, 202 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE whereas there is only one in about fifty thousand that the larger coefficient of the second day is accidental. Nevertheless, the posi- tive correlation on the eleventh day may be significant. If so, it presumably means that a rise of temperature is accompanied by conditions favorable to the development of pneumonia, so that an unusually large number of people die about eleven days after- ward. Here again we appear to have a curious contradiction between the effect of relatively high temperature and of the change toward such a temperature. This contrast appears so constantly and consistently that its reality can scarcely be doubted. One of the clearest and most convincing features of this in- vestigation of daily changes of temperature is its unequivocal character. In all three diagrams (D, E, and F) the high coeffi- cients are either higher or more numerous than in the corre- sponding diagrams for mean temperature (A, B, and C). This agrees with several other lines of evidence, such as the sensitive- ness of people in monotonous climates, in suggesting that varia- bility of temperature not only from season to season but from day to day may be almost as important as the mean temperature itself. Such slight evidence as is yet available also suggests that variability in other respects such as sunshine, rainfall, moisture, and wind may have an appreciable effect upon health. The em- phasis thus given to variability as a distinct factor, apart from the conditions which vary, is of much significance in connection with changes of climate and the relation of climate to the dis- tribution of civilization. It confirms the conclusions derived from our study of factories, general death rates, influenza, pneu- monia, and operations, and is itself confirmed by strong evidence which is yet to come. Turning now to relative humidity, we find that diagram G is completely negative. During the years in question the relative humidity of the air had no appreciable effect upon the health of children under five years old in New York. This is true even HEALTH AND WEATHER 203 when the effects of mean temperature and changes of tempera- ture are eliminated. Among older people (diagram H) the same is true so far as any immediate effect is concerned, for days to 6 have irregular and insignificant coefficients. From the sixth to the fourteenth day after any given condition of relative hu- midity, however, there is a slight but persistent positive correla- tion every day. On two days this rises to almost four times the probable error. This suggests that in winter high humidity may possibly be favorable to the contraction of diseases from which people die a week or two later. A similar suggestion in respect to influenza has already been discussed. The pneumonia diagram (I), however, has a different aspect. None of its coefficients are large enough to be significant, but the fact that the first seven are all positive gives a hint that high relative humidity may have a slightly unfavorable effect upon pneumonia patients. It is most perplexing to find that different sets of data give different indications as to the relation between atmospheric humidity and health. The present investigation with its almost negative results, but with a slight suggestion that damp air facilitates the transmission of harmful micro-organisms, agrees with the experiments of the New York Ventilation Commission, and with the results obtained by the Committee on the Atmos- phere and Man in its work on influenza. Opposed to this are the results of what seem to be equally reliable investigations per- taining to deaths after operations, to the death rate from pneu- monia by months as set forth by Greenburg, to Besson's inquiry into the weekly death rate in Paris, and to the monthly death rate where millions of people were studied as described in World Power and Evolution. Moreover, a new investigation, shortly to be described, points even more strongly toward atmospheric humidity as an important agent in promoting health. A possible explanation of this apparent contradiction may be that humid- ity affects people in two ways, directly through the skin, nerves, and lungs ; and indirectly through minute organisms that bear 204 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE disease. The disease-bearing organisms being very short lived, are quickly influenced by variations in atmospheric moisture. Hence a day or two of unusually high relative humidity may be enough to give them an opportunity to produce disease. Man, on the other hand, may be influenced more slowly so that the bene- ficial effect of moisture upon him becomes apparent only when he is subjected to moderately moist conditions for some time. Thus relatively long oscillations in health may arise through the effect of atmospheric moisture upon man, and short oscillation* through the effect upon the bringers of disease, and the two may easily be of opposite character. We now come to what seems to me a most conclusive study as to the general effect of the weather upon health. In order to gain a comprehensive view of the variations in this effect from place to place and likewise from season to season, I have made a fresh investigation of the deaths each month from 1900 to 1915 in thirty-three cities of the United States. Every city with over 100,000 population in 1910 has been used so far as mortality data are available. Each city and each month of the year has been treated as a separate unit, the Januaries, Februaries, and so on being divided into two equal groups on the basis of each of the following climatic factors : (1) Mean daily temperature. (2) Mean daily relative humidity (average of 8 a.m. and 8p.m.). (3) Variability or storminess (number of storms whose centers passed within two hundred miles of the given city, allow- ing double weight to those within one hundred miles). (4) Wind (total number of miles per month). Five cities only. Data for the wind were investigated only at New York, Balti- more, Chicago, St. Louis, and San Francisco. The Chicago data are doubtful because the growth of the city appears gradually HEALTH AND WEATHER 205 to have cut off the wind from the Weather Bureau Station and caused an apparent decline in windiness. An example will show how the data were used. The eight warm- est Januaries in New York averaged 6.0 F. warmer than the eight coldest, and had fewer deaths by 0.6 per cent. In February the excess of temperature in the eight warmest months amounted to 6.5 and their death rate was 4.1 per cent less than that of the cooler months. In March the corresponding figures were 6.4 and 9.7 per cent; in April 3.8 and 4.5 per cent; in May, on the contrary, an excess of 3.5 in temperature was accom- panied by a death rate 1.5 per cent greater in the warm months than in the cool months, while in July, although the eight warm months averaged only 2.8 above the eight cooler months, the excess in their death rate rose to 14.2 per cent. The use of this method brings out many interesting facts on which we cannot now dwell. It eliminates entirely all complica- tions due to the seasons, for each month stands by itself and can be compared with any other month. It likewise shows nothing as to the general effect of the seasonal variations of either weather or mortality. It merely shows how the departures of any par- ticular climatic factor from the normal for that particular month affect the death rate. Boston, for example, is thus found to be benefited in summer by the coolness of its damp east winds ; dampness without coolness is the bane of New Haven in summer ; New York, by reason partly of its great size and partly of its fine climate, is unusually regular in its responses to the weather, but for some unexplained and possibly accidental reason is averse to storms in midsummer as well as in midwinter. Although Baltimore is hot in summer, it suffers little harm from humidity even when that factor runs high. Baltimore is likewise benefited at all seasons if it has more than its usual allowance of storms, while Boston, being far north, normally has so many storms that it is better off during the less stormy months except in summer. Chicago, on the other hand, is benefited by storms except during o CO A . Q *H O 4) 3 Q H O 208 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the coldest months ; it is likewise benefited by high relative hu- midity except in the autumn, but curiously enough it is not bene- fited by high temperature in winter, perhaps because the warm months experience dry southwest winds, bare ground, and dust. Somewhat the same is true in Pittsburgh and Denver. Cleveland and San Francisco have climates of such a type that departures from the normal produce relatively little effect, whereas in cities like St. Paul and Minneapolis in the north and St. Louis and Cincinnati farther south the effects are much greater. In southerly places like Nashville and Memphis the effect of de- partures from the normal weather is especially great. Presum- ably it is still greater in the far south, but Atlanta is the only other southern city for which data are available. In Figure 18 (a and b) the thirty-three cities have been com- bined into geographical groups. Each city is weighted according to its population as follows : 100,000 to 200,000 = 1 ; 200,000 to 400,000^2; 400,000 to 1,000,000 = 3; 1,000,000 to 2,000,000 = 4 ; over 2,000,000 = 5. The curved lines indicate the extent to which the death rates in the eight warmer, moister, or stormier Januaries, Februaries, and so forth, from 1900 to 1915 differed from the corresponding death rates in the eight cooler, drier, and less stormy Januaries and other months. The figures in the scales beside the diagrams indicate departures in percentages of the normals. The normals are the estimated num- bers of deaths that each place would have experienced per month in any given year if the number of deaths changed regularly in response to the growth of the city and the improvements in medi- cal practice without regard to weather, seasons, epidemics, and so forth. The method of getting the normals is explained in World Power and Evolution. In Figure 18 and the other figures of the same kind all the curves have been smoothed by the formula a + 2b + c b, HEALTH AND WEATHER 209 which is a common way of eliminating the confusing minor ir- regularities which arise because of the small number of years for which data are available. In the figure the heavily shaded areas mean that the months with relatively high temperature, high humidity, high storminess, or high winds had lower death rates than the months in which the weather factors stood lower. For example New York, Philadelphia, and New Haven form a group of cities lying within a distance of about one hundred and fifty miles and having similar climates in spite of individual idiosyncrasies. In the first column of Figure 18, diagram B shows that in these cities the months of January, February, and especially March are too cold, for in each month the death rate in the eight warmest years was from 3 to 5 per cent lower than in the eight coldest, as appears from the dark shading. In April the average temperature was about right, for the curve crosses the zero line ; in that month the bad effect produced by weather that is a little too cool is balanced by the corresponding effect of high temperature during the same month. As summer ad- vances the warm months begin to have a disadvantage, as is indicated by the dotted shading. The eight warmer Julies had an average death rate about 6 per cent greater than that of the cooler Julies even in the smoothed curve ; in the unsmoothed curve this rises to 10.4. Inasmuch as the eight warmest Julies averaged only about 3 warmer than the coolest eight, each degree of excessive temperature raised the death rate more than 3 per cent. Passing on to the second column of Figure 18 it appears that high relative humidity is beneficial to New York and its neigh- bors throughout the winter, and especially in April when an excess of 6 per cent in relative humidity is accompanied by a diminution of 6 per cent in the death rate. During the three summer months the highest humidities do harm, the smoothed maximum excess of deaths being 4 per cent while the unsmoothed figures are 5.8 per cent accompanying an excess of 6.4 per cent 210 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE in the relative humidity. The duration of the period when hu- midity is harmful is short. During the autumn and early winter the nearness of the curve to the zero line indicates that it makes little difference whether those months are relatively dry or moist, perhaps because the general conditions of health are so good that people can resist extremes which might harm them at less favorable seasons. Nevertheless, the damper rather than the drier months were the best. In the third column of Figure 18 the storms of the New York group of cities appear to have had less influence than either the temperature or the humidity. In the late winter and spring the more stormy months were the most healthful; in summer the less stormy ones had a very slight advantage, too small to be significant ; the autumn again was like the late winter, while November and December were like June and July. This particu- lar curve happens to be one where the effect of storms is at a minimum. The most important thing about it is that the dark shading is much more extensive than the light, which means that on the whole the stormiest months were times of better health than those that were less stormy. In the right-hand column of Figure 18, where the effect of the wind is shown, the upper diagram is based only on New York. It is very symmetrical, and indicates that in winter high winds are accompanied by a high death rate, while in summer they are accompanied by a low death rate. The question at once arises whether the four types of curves in Figure 18 really represent the effect of the individual climatic factors or whether each curve is compounded of the effects of all four factors. For example, are high winds really favorable in summer, or do they merely appear to be so because they are accompanied by low temperature or low humidity or some other favorable condition? The answer is found in Figure 19. The solid curves there are the same as those which we have just been examining for the New York group of cities in Figure 18, but HEALTH AND WEATHER 211 are plotted on a larger vertical scale. The dotted lines are the same curves corrected to allow for the other climatic factors. In making the corrections it was assumed that temperature is the most important climatic factor, and is followed by humidity, JFMAMJJyASOVDJ 5 Temperature Relative Humidity -2 -3 -f Storms 3 z 1 o Wind New York -1 -2 Figure 19. Correction for Effect of Other Climatic Factors storms, and wind in this order, for this is what is commonly supposed, and it is supported by the investigation here de- scribed. The method of making the corrections was as follows : Knowing how much the temperature and the death rate in the moister months differed from those in the drier months, it was 212 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE easy to determine how much effect a given difference of tempera- ture produces. Since we know the difference in temperature be- tween the moister and drier months, it is possible to make allowance for this difference month by month and thereby elimi- nate from the humidity curve the effect of temperature. When the humidity curve had thus been corrected, the storm curve was corrected in the same way on the basis not only of the tempera- ture curve but of the corrected humidity curve. Next the humid- ity curve was corrected to allow for the effect of storms, and the temperature curve to allow for the effect of both storms and humidity. Finally, the wind curve was corrected to allow for variations in all three of the other factors. The result was the dotted lines in Figure 19. At the top of the diagram the cor- rected and uncorrected lines showing the effect of temperature upon the death rate are practically alike. The next pair of lines, those for humidity, are almost alike, but allowance for the other factors seems slightly to reduce the importance of humidity in the spring and raise it in summer. In the third pair of lines the corrections seem to lower the general level a trifle, thus making it appear that storms are a bit more beneficial than appeared at first sight. Finally, the corrected and uncorrected curves for the winds are completely different. The good effect in summer has practically disappeared in the corrected curve. If the data for the wind had been available from New Haven and Philadelphia, as well as New York, the smoothing of the wind curve might pos- sibly have been still more complete. As things now stand, the cor- rections seem to indicate that in Figure 18, in spite of minor details due to other factors, the apparent effects of temperature, humidity, and storms represent approximately the real effects, and would represent them still more closely if a larger number of cities were averaged together as will be done in Figure 20. As for the wind, it may be that high winds in winter have some direct effect in raising the death rate, but in summer practically all of their effect appears to arise from the conditions of tern- HEALTH AND WEATHER 213 perature, humidity, and storms, or variability which accompany them. Let us return now to Figure 18. In the left-hand column the groups of cities east of the Mississippi behave almost as one would expect. Except for Rochester and Buffalo (D), which appear to be practically never too hot, all the diagrams are heavily shaded in winter and lightly shaded in summer, thus indicating that the winters from Tennessee and northern Georgia to Minneapolis and Boston are too cold and the sum- mers too hot for the best health. In the center of this area, to be sure, group E (Cleveland, Toledo, and Detroit), group F (Pittsburgh, Columbus, and Indianapolis), and group G (At- lanta, Nashville, and Memphis), show a curious depression in summer, as if the harmful effect of hot weather was somehow inhibited, perhaps because the hot winds are dry so that the bad effects of high humidity are mitigated when the temperature rises. West of the Mississippi all of the groups (K to N) suggest that while hot summer weather is generally bad except on the cool Pacific coast, unusually warm winter weather is also often harmful. This is presumably because the warm months are gen- erally dry ; and our monthly data seem to show that dryness is almost always harmful in cold weather. That this last statement is true seems to be abundantly veri- fied by the second column in Figure 18. Here there is no such equal distribution of heavy and light shading as in the diagrams showing the effect of temperature. On the contrary, almost every individual diagram displays a greater area of heavy shading than of light, and some of the diagrams such as C, G, and K have practically no light shading. In general the amount of heavy shading, that is, the good effect of atmospheric moisture, in- creases as one goes from the moister and cooler parts of the country to those that are warmer and drier. It reaches a maxi- mum in diagram L for Denver and Spokane, the two cities in our list where the atmospheric moisture is least. This clearly 214 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE means that in practically all parts of the United States, so far as data are available, and especially in the drier parts, the health of the inhabitants would be materially improved if there were more atmospheric moisture. This clear-cut and apparently un- equivocal result agrees with the study of deaths set forth in World Power and Evolution, and with the study of the death rate after operations. The contrast between these three lines of evidence, on the one hand, and the results of our investigation of daily deaths in New York, together with the work of the Ventila- tion Commission, on the other, is the reason for our suggestion that humidity has two diverse and opposed effects. It seems to be beneficial in its direct effect, except at temperatures above the optimum, and harmful in its indirect effect through bacteria. Much of what has been said of atmospheric moisture is like- wise true of storms, as appears in the third column of Figure 18. Notice how largely the heavy shading predominates. Note also that it is scarce in northern groups of cities such as B, H, and M, but increases as one goes southward until in groups G, J, and especially C practically every month of the year shows a lower death rate when storms are relatively abundant than when they are few. Here, just as in the case of humidity, the regions which have few storms, like those which have little at- mospheric moisture, give heavily shaded diagrams because an increase in the number of storms is an advantage to health at practically all seasons. But places like Boston, which are exposed not only to many storms but to strong oceanic winds, may get too many storms in the winter. The question of the effect of storminess is so important that I have prepared Figure 20 to show what happens when stormy periods last several months. The upper diagram in each case shows the conditions when the storms of a given month are com- pared with the deaths of that month. In the second diagram the months of the sixteen years used in our study have been grouped into halves according to the number of storms not only in the HEALTH AND WEATHER 215 month when deaths occurred, but in the preceding month. In the lower diagram the storminess of three consecutive months has been compared with the deaths in the third month. To begin with Boston, a relatively high degree of storminess lasting only a single month is slightly beneficial in summer and again, curi- ously enough, in winter, but this may be a mere accident due to the shortness of our record. If the stormy period lasts two months the good effect of storms is much increased. In fact, Boston's health would apparently be distinctly improved if the city could have frequent periods of relatively high storminess lasting two months during the summer, but not during the late winter. The lightly shaded area in the autumn is so small that it may be acci- dental. If the periods of storminess last three months, however, Boston gets too much of them and the death rate rises markedly. In other words, Boston seems to lie close to the fortunate level where it gets neither too many nor too few storms in the long run, although in the more extreme periods it gets too many, just as in milder periods it does not get enough. Contrast Boston with Chicago in Figure 20. A single month of more than the average storminess helps Chicago a good deal during all seasons except midwinter. But two successive months of unusual storminess, and especially three, do harm at prac- tically all seasons. In other words, an increase in storminess hurts Chicago more than Boston. Nevertheless, both cities evi- dently profit greatly by the fact that they have many storms, as appears when they are compared with cities farther south. In New York, for example, Figure 20 shows that increased storminess during periods of more than one month is beneficial in summer but not in winter, while Seattle is benefited by increased and prolonged storminess at practically all seasons. This simply means that New York, with less severe storms than Chicago or Boston, would profit by a mild increase in storminess. On the other hand, Seattle, with far less storminess than the other cities would be better off to have decidedly more. Boston AMJJyASOND,! CJiieogo JFM A M J Jy A 6 O N D New Yorlt Figure 20. Excess or Deficiency of Deaths in Relation to Stormy Periods Lasting One Month (Upper Diagrams), Two Months (Middle), and Three Months (Lower), 1900-1915 HEALTH AND WEATHER 217 At the bottom of Figure 20 Baltimore and Washington, which are treated as a single unit, and Memphis on the Mississippi River in Tennessee present still a third type. At all seasons their stormiest months are the most healthful, for these cities lie toward the southern edge of the storm belt. They have far less stormy weather than New York, Chicago, and Boston, but more than Seattle. During the years under discussion all these places were especially stormy in March. They are benefited by prolonged periods of storminess in summer and autumn, but cannot stand such periods in the winter and spring. Too much emphasis must not be placed on the minor details of any of the diagrams in Figure 20, especially that of a small city like Mem- phis, for the number of years included in our data is small. Nevertheless, there seems to be little question that storminess has an important effect upon health. In a belt of country ex- tending from New York and Boston westward to Chicago, the beneficial effects of storminess are greatest. To the north of that belt increased storminess appears to have a harmful effect upon health; to the south the present degree of storminess is not enough on an average, and a higher degree regularly causes an improvement in health at most seasons. In concluding this chapter let us turn to Figure 21. Here the data for all of our thirty-three American cities have been com- bined into a single diagram. This shows the effect produced by an unusually high condition of any one of the four climatic fac- tors upon the death rate in the whole northern United States together with the Pacific coast. In using such a large area there is great opportunity for opposed conditions in different regions to cancel one another, but this cancellation is far from complete. Before we interpret Figure 21 let us consider for a moment the degree to which the diagrams may be the result of chance. In most such cases it is the custom for mathematicians to compute the probable error by means of a formula. Our method, however, whereby cities are weighted according to their population, and 218 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the departures are reckoned from normals which pertain to the year instead of the individual months, would cause such compu- tations to take an excessive amount of time. Accordingly I have calculated the data for four months exactly as in the diagrams, except that pure chance has been allowed to control the choice of months for each of the two groups of eight years into which the sixteen years have been divided. The smoothed results for JFMAMJJyASONDJ Temperature Relative Humidity Storms Wind Figure 21. Net Effect of Weather in the United States comparison with Figure 21 are January 0.47, April 0.40, July 0.72, October 0.79. In each of the four diagrams the extremes are four to seven times as great as the accidental variations thus obtained. This fact, together with the systematic character of the results, makes it practically certain that we are dealing with real relationships and not with accidental coincidences. If this be accepted, our diagrams in Figure 21 show that in the United States as a whole, excluding the South except in HEALTH AND WEATHER 219 California, a certain amount of harm is done to health in winter by low temperature, but not so much as one would expect. The high temperatures of summer do much more harm. The effects of the wind seem to be very clear, but are probably due largely to the conditions of temperature, humidity, and storminess which accompany the winds. Now for the most surprising fact ; lack of moisture does almost as much harm to the United States in winter as does low temperature, while in the spring it does only a little less than does high temperature in summer. At all seasons the United States as a whole, omitting the parts for which we have no data, has poorer health in unusually dry weather than in that which is unusually moist. Almost the same thing is true of storms. The country is better off when stormi- ness is unusually abundant, except in the late fall. In Figure 21 the average departures from the zero line, regardless of whether the departures are positive or negative, are as follows : tempera- ture 4.09 per cent, relative humidity 3.96, and storminess 3.60. These figures seem to represent approximately the relative im- portance of the three great climatic factors. Thus in spite of certain puzzling facts as to humidity, the general result of our study of health in relation to the weather is to confirm the re- sults of our previous study of efficiency in factories. The same conditions of temperature, humidity, and variability which cause people to work quickly or slowly in the ordinary affairs of life seem also to cause their health to be good or poor. CHAPTER X THE IDEAL CLIMATE WE are frequently told that the Riviera or southern Cali- fornia has an ideal climate. Florida lays claim to it m winter, the Alps in summer. Two of the few regions which rarely assert their preeminence in this respect are Boston with its east winds and London with its fogs. Yet in many ways they have a strong claim to high rank. It all depends upon what we mean by "ideal." For rest and recreation a warm, equable climate is doubtless most delightful; for a fishing or climbing trip some- thing quite different is desirable. For most people the really essential thing in life is the ordinary work of every day. Hence, the climate which is best for work may in the long run claim to be the most nearly ideal. But such climates are also the ones that are best for health. Hence they are the ones which people will ultimately choose in the largest numbers. The few disagree- able features at certain seasons are no worse than the shiver at the beginning of a cold plunge. On the basis of both work and health, the best climate would apparently be one in which the mean temperature rarely falls below the mental optimum of perhaps 38, or rises above the physical optimum of about 64. From this point of view the most ideal conditions would seem to be found where the tempera- ture for the year as a whole averages not far from 51, as at London, Paris, New York, and Peking. In four chief portions of the globe, the winter temperature averages not far from 38, and that of summer not far from 64. The first of these is Eng- land. At London the thermometer averages 38 in January and THE IDEAL CLIMATE 221 63 in July, while at Liverpool the figures are 39 and 60. If an average of 51 at all seasons were ideal, southwestern Ireland, with a range of from 45 to 59, and the Hebrides, from 42 to 55, would be more ideal than London. On the continent, where the seasonal variation is greater than in Britain, the length of the relatively unfavorable periods with temperatures above 65 and below 38 also increases. A second region where the temperature conditions approach the ideal is the Pacific coast of the northern United States and southern British Columbia. Seattle, averaging 39 in January and 64 in July has practically the same temperature as Lon- don. Southward the seasonal range decreases. San Francisco, averaging 49 in January and 59 in September after the cool summer fogs have passed away, may claim in many ways to be ideal. Still farther south the temperatures of Los Angeles and San Diego, 53 or 54 in January and 69 in August, fluctuate about the physical optimum and would be ideal for physical ac- tivity if mean temperature were the only criterion. The mental optimum, however, is lower than the temperature of all except the unusually cold days, and variations from day to day are rare. A short distance inland the Californian climate becomes less favorable than on the coast, for the average in summer at Fresno, for example, is 82. Even though the heat is mitigated by low humidity, the continuance of such high temperature causes people to feel indisposed to activity. England and the Pacific coast owe their climatic excellence largely to the fact that ocean winds from the west blow freely over them. Two regions in the southern hemisphere enjoy the same advantage, namely, New Zealand and part of South Amer- ica, including southern Chili and portions of Patagonia. We are apt to think of these South American regions as sparsely settled places of little importance. This is true, for the present, but it is not because the climate prevents activity. The climate, to be sure, is a drawback, but the harmful feature is not the tempera- 222 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ture, but the rainfall. Plants, not man, are the chief sufferers. Unlike our other three regions, this part of the world has a deficient rainfall except in the west, and there high mountains hinder settlement. In spite of this, the few small portions of Patagonia that permit profitable agriculture are making prog- ress and would doubtless do so rapidly if not hampered by re- moteness, by the absence of railroads, and by the social handi- cap of their aborigines. It must not be inferred that the climates of Patagonia, New Zealand, England, and the Pacific coast of the United States are necessarily ideal. Mean temperature is not the only impor- tant factor. Among other things the relative humidity must be considered. The deficiency of moisture in Patagonia not only is disastrous economically, but, to judge from the preceding studies, it lessens man's energy. A similar effect is produced by excess of moisture, and thus harm is done in Ireland and western Scotland, which would otherwise be almost as fortunate as Eng- land. New Zealand, the central Pacific coast of North America, and England itself are sometimes unduly damp for long periods, but nevertheless enjoy a relative humidity of not far from 70 per cent much of the time. Other regions, however, such as the eastern United States and central Europe, seem to be more favored in this respect. The change of temperature from day to day, as we have seen, seems to be as important as relative humidity, and must accord- ingly be considered more fully. Its effect on human activity seems to be second only to that of the mean temperature of the seasons. The intensity and number of daily changes depend upon two chief factors, first, the range of temperature from the warmest to the coldest part of the year, and second, the number of cyclonic storms. Where the winters are cold and the summers hot, the changes from day to day are also extreme. For instance, in the Dakotas, where the mean temperatures of January and July differ by 60 F., a change of equal magnitude may take THE IDEAL CLIMATE 223 place in twenty-four hours. On the other hand, in a place like the Congo, where the difference between the coldest and warmest months is only three or four degrees, the days are correspond- ingly uniform. The whole matter is illustrated by maps in many physical geographies and in such publications as Bartholomew's Meteorological A tlas. The parts of the world where the change of seasons favors a highly advantageous degree of change from one day to the next include most of North America, but omit Florida, the Pacific coast of the United States, and the regions from the Mexican border southward. All of central and eastern Europe is also included except parts of Italy and Greece. A large area in North Africa and a small area in the south of that conti- nent also rank high, as do central Australia and the part of South America which includes central Argentina. Finally, all except the southern parts of Asia lie within the high area. Thus this particular favorable condition occurs not only in many regions whose climate is also good from other standpoints, but in a much larger number whose general climatic conditions are decidedly unfavorable. This is not surprising, for the beneficial effect of pronounced changes of temperature from day to day is often nullified by great heat in summer or extreme cold in winter. Moreover, the seasonal range of temperature forms only one of the two factors which determine the amount of stimulation derived from changes of temperature from day to day. The other factor is the number of cyclonic storms. By this, as has already been explained, we mean the ordinary storms which produce our changes of weather from day to day in the United States and Europe. Probably the storms are more im- portant than the range of temperature from season to season, for they bring rain, humidity, changes in the winds, and all sorts of stimulating variations. The world's stormiest region, so far as known, includes the Great Lakes region of the United States and southern Canada. Around this center there is an area of great storminess extending southward approximately to Mary- 224 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE land and Kansas, and northwestward through the Dakotas to Alberta. Eastward it includes New England and the Maritime provinces, while northward it quickly disappears. To the south the storminess diminishes gradually, so that Florida has a moderate degree of variability in winter but not in summer. Southern California is the least stormy part of the United States. In Europe the very stormy regions include Britain, most of France, Germany, parts of Scandinavia, and the north- ern part of Italy, together with western Austria and the Baltic region. In Asia, Japan is the only place where cyclonic storms are at all numerous. The lands of the southern hemisphere gen- erally have few storms. New Zealand is the chief exception, al- though there they do not cause such great changes of tempera- ture as in America and Europe. The extreme southern tip of South America is likewise stormy, but its storms do not cause much variability. On the contrary, they give rise to a monotony of wind and clouds which is extremely deadening, according to the testimony of those who have lived in such places as Tierra del Fuego or the Falkland Islands. Farther north, in central Argentina, there is a moderate number of storms, comparable to those in the southern United States, and their effect is dis- tinctly favorable. We are now prepared to estimate the relative stimulating power of the various climates of the world. In England, for ex- ample, the mean temperature of the seasons and the degree of storminess are both highly favorable, while the seasonal changes are only moderate. Germany is above medium in temperature, and high in seasonal changes and storminess. In this respect, it resembles the northeastern United States and southern Canada. Japan is similar except that it is somewhat too warm and damp. The coast of British Columbia and of the neighboring states is highly favorable in mean temperature, and medium in storminess and seasonal changes. Around San Francisco, the mean tempera- ture is still better, but both seasonal changes and storms are THE IDEAL CLIMATE 225 mild. In compensation for this, however, there are frequent changes of temperature because fogs blow in from the ocean, and are quickly succeeded by the warm, bright weather which generally characterizes the interior. Farther south where the fogs cease, the conditions become less favorable from the point of view of the changes from one day to another, although the mean temperature of the seasons still remains advantageous. The chief defect of the climate of the California coast is that it is too uniformly stimulating. Perhaps the constant activity which it incites may be a factor in causing nervous disorders. When allowance is made for the fact that California's urban population is relatively smaller than that of states like Massa- chusetts and New York, insanity appears to be even more prevalent than in those states. Moreover, the cities of the Cali- fornia coast have the highest rate of suicide. In 1922 four California cities led the list in suicides, the number per 100,000 population being : San Diego 47.8, Sacramento 37.9, San Fran- cisco 30.4, and Los Angeles 30.3, against about 15 in the eastern cities. Possibly these facts may be connected with the constant stimulation of the favorable temperature and the lack of relaxa- tion through variations from season to season and from day to day, although other factors must also play a part. The people of California may perhaps be likened to horses which are urged to the limit so that some of them become unduly tired and break down. In the same way the people of the eastern and central United States are more nervous and active than those of Europe but not necessarily more efficient because of still different climatic handicaps. They are alternately stimulated and relaxed by frequent changes from day to day, and in this are like horses that are well driven. In the spring and autumn, however, the combined effect of ideal temperature and highly invigorating daily changes spurs them to an astonishing degree of effort. Then comes the hot summer or the cold winter, either of which is debilitating. People do not diminish their activity at once, espe- 226 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ciallj in the winter. They draw on their nervous energy, and thus exhaust themselves. They are like horses which pull on the bit, and when urged a little break into a run, straining themselves by their extreme speed. Then they are pulled up so suddenly that they are thrown back on their haunches and injured. In Ger- many somewhat the same conditions prevail, although not to so great an extent. England apparently comes nearer to the ideal than almost any other place. The climate is stimulating at all times, both by reason of abundant storms and because of a moderate seasonal range. It never, however, reaches such ex- tremes as to induce the nervous tension which prevails so largely in the United States. In strong contrast to these highly favored regions are such places as the center of Asia, where the winters are depressingly cold and the summers unduly hot. The range from season to season is apparently helpful, but its good effects are largely nullified by the infrequency of storms. Day succeeds day with no apparent change. In the desert of Takla-Makan in Chinese Turkestan in the fall of 1905, 1 found that one of the most sur- prising features was the way in which winter came upon us unawares. Each morning the thermometer stood a trifle lower than the preceding morning, but there was never any change such as that which we so often experience in America when the first severe frost suddenly comes after a series of days as warm as summer. Frost at last began to prevail at night, but not until we found the water frozen hard in the morning did we realize that winter was upon us. So it goes, month after month, with deaden- ing monotony. Yet when a storm does come the change is often much more extreme than in more oceanic regions. It is frequently so great that its value as a stimulus is much diminished. Tropical regions suffer even greater disadvantages than do places like the center of Asia. Not only is the temperature un- favorably high, but there are practically no cyclonic storms except in portions where a few hurricanes occur each year. THE IDEAL CLIMATE 227 Thunder storms, to be sure, are abundant, but they rarely bring any important change of temperature. Moreover, the seasonal range from the warmest to the coldest month is generally less than the difference between day and night. Day after day dis- plays no appreciable variation from its predecessor. The uni- formity of the climate seems to be more deadly than its heat. Such uniformity, perhaps as much as the high temperature and high humidity, may be one of the most potent causes of the physical debility which affects so many white men within the tropics, and which manifests itself in weaknesses such as drunk- enness, immorality, anger, and laziness. Even in tropical high- lands the same deadening monotony prevails, although to a less degree than in the lowlands. Such monotony is perhaps the condition which will do most to prevent the white man from living there permanently for generation after generation. His general health may not seem to suffer, but if he works hard he is in great danger of breaking down nervously. The temperature of the highlands may be highly stimulating. There are many places where the mean temperature during every month in the year is within a few degrees of either the physical or mental optimum, or of their average. At Quito in Ecuador, for example, the cold- est month, November, averages 54.3 F., and the warmest months, February and September, 55. Nowhere within the tropics, however, are there any regions which enjoy the physical optimum at one season and the mental optimum at another. If we are justified in associating a high rate of insanity on both the Atlantic and Pacific coasts of the United States with the peculiar climatic conditions, we should expect that white men in tropical regions at high altitudes would suffer still more in the same way, or else would become inert, but no figures seem to be available to determine this point. We might proceed to discuss scores of ways in which a knowl- edge of the exact effects of climate may assist in the under- standing of historic events, or help in guiding future develop- 228 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE ment. As a step in this direction let us construct a map of the world showing the degree of energy which we should expect among normal Europeans in various regions on the basis of cli- mate. This map will be based entirely on our studies of work among factory operatives and students, and I shall leave the description of it unchanged from the first edition of this book except for the correction of an error. Later we shall test this map of "climatic energy" by one of "climatic health" based on our studies of mortality and disease. Both maps are determined by (1) the mean temperature month by month, (2) the amount of change from one day to another, and (3) the relative humid- ity. The conditions which prevail at various seasons in the east- ern United States duplicate those of almost every portion of the globe. There are hot, dry days like those of the Sahara ; hot, damp days like those in the Amazon forests ; cold days like those on the great ice-sheet of Greenland, and days of almost every other description. At this point we must make an assumption which cannot be tested until vastly more data have been col- lected. Let us assume that the continuance of a given condition produces the same effect as its temporary occurrence. For ex- ample, in Connecticut our measurements of the effect of days having a mean temperature of 75 are based on occasional days scattered through the summers of several years. Only in rare cases do four or five days of such extreme temperature follow in succession without interruption by more moderate weather. The actual figures show that the first hot day does not greatly diminish people's energy, for the human body is able to resist for a while and to carry the impetus of previous good conditions into the first part of a bad period. After two or three days, how- ever, the heat takes hold on people and makes them inefficient, or even causes some to collapse. If such weather continued for months we should become somewhat accustomed to it, and the period of collapse would be past. Just what the rate of work would then be we cannot yet determine. It would almost certainly THE IDEAL CLIMATE 229 be slower than on the first hot day, but it would probably be faster than on the third or fourth. Because of this uncer- tainty we are obliged for the present to assume that it would be equal to the average of a number of first days and a much smaller number of second, third, fourth, and so on. Having made this assumption, but recognizing that it needs testing, we may go on to construct our map. We must remember that it is not supposed to be a map of the actual energy displayed by the people of various places, but of the energy that we should expect among Europeans if they lived in these places and were influ- enced as are the people of the eastern United States. In making such a map it is fortunate that the most important factor is also the one most carefully tabulated by climatologists, and for which our investigations of energy give the most un- equivocal results. The mean temperature for every month in the year is given for about 1100 stations in all parts of the world in Hann's Klimatologie. The third curve from the top in Figure 10, it will be remembered, shows the average efficiency which would be expected at any given temperature on the basis of the work of factory operatives in Connecticut and New York. A table inserted as an appendix to this book shows the value for each degree of temperature according to the centigrade scale, the maximum being reckoned as 100 at a temperature of 15 C. or 59 F. To determine the effect of mean temperature upon human activity we simply take from Hann the mean temperature of each month, and then from the table in the Appendix, or from the curve in Figure 10, find the corresponding relative efficiency. Then we add the values for all the months. If every month had an average temperature of 59, with a corresponding relative efficiency of 100, the efficiency for the place in question would be 1200. As a matter of fact this is never reached, but London stands at 1196.6, San Francisco at 1198.6, and Quito in Ecua- dor at 1198.9. The worst place is Massaua near the southern end of the Red Sea, where the figure is 1070. 230 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE The next process in constructing a map of climatic energy is to determine the effect of changes of temperature from day to day. Unfortunately exact statistics are not available in most regions, and we are obliged to employ an approximation. Since changes from day to day depend chiefly upon the seasonal range of temperature and the number of storms, I have combined the two, giving equal weight to each, and giving the two together approximately one half the weight assigned to seasonal tempera- ture. That is, the difference between Quito and Massaua, as stated in the last paragraph, is 128.9. This represents the maxi- mum effect of the seasons, so far as the average temperature is concerned. The maximum effect of the seasons as far as changes from day to day are concerned is reckoned as 30, and the maxi- mum effect of storminess on the same basis is also reckoned as 30. Since highly extreme conditions are not favorable, I have assumed that no seasonal change beyond 30 C., or 54 F., is of value, and also that changes below 7 C. (19.4 F.) or above 23 C. (73.4 F.) are of no value. In other words, if the range from the mean temperature of the coldest to the warmest month is from below 7 C. to above 23 C., it is reckoned as having a value of 30, just as it would be if 7 were the lowest point and 23 the highest. If the range should be from 4 C. to 16 C., it would be reckoned as having a value of 12, while if it were from 20 C. to 28 C., the value would be only 3, because the extremely hot weather above 23 would scarcely be stimulating even if there were slight changes from day to day. In the same wav extreme storminess does not produce an effect in propor- tion to the number of storms. One storm may succeed another so rapidly that the weather ceases to have sufficient variety, and becomes dull and lowering all the time. This is the case at Cape Horn, and also in certain parts of the North American Great Lakes region in winter. Accordingly a storminess of 20 centers per year according to Kullmer's scale which means far more than 20 storms is reckoned as the optimum. Greater stormi- THE IDEAL CLIMATE 231 ness is held to have the same stimulating value as 20 centers, while everything lower is counted proportionally. The whole matter is so technical that it cannot be understood without de- tailed explanations. These are now unnecessary, since the very simple method to be described shortly gives an almost identical but more accurate map. Humidity has not been considered, because the necessary figures are not available. In most of the cooler parts of the world it would make little difference, although a few unduly damp places like Ireland, or excessively dry regions like Chinese Turkestan would be lower than now appears. The chief differ- ence would be in the warm portions of the world. Agra in north- ern India, for instance, now has a lower rank than Bombay and Calcutta, but if allowance were made for humidity this would probably be reversed, for Agra is pleasantly dry much of the year. The same reversal would probably occur between dry Khartum and wet Equatorville on the Congo. Arizona and other desert portions of the United States would also make a better appearance than on the present map. It must not be forgotten, however, that our data for New England show that extreme dryness does more harm than extreme humidity. This, however, does not apply to high temperatures. Under such conditions great humidity is undoubtedly most debilitating. Yet even when the air is hot, it may be too dry. In such a place as Death Valley, in summer with the thermometer at 100 to 135 in the shade, it is almost impossible to drink enough water to preserve normal physiological conditions. Even a brief period of physical ac- tivity gives rise to much discomfort, and people who stay through the summer are in danger of suffering permanent injury to health. Our knowledge of the effect of both extreme humidity and extreme dryness is unfortunately still qualitative rather than quantitative. Some day, however, exact figures for all the vari- ous climatic elements will be obtainable, and we shall construct 232 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE a map showing the actual efficiency to be expected in every part of the world. It will be so accurate that the manufacturer, for example, who contemplates establishing a factory, will be able to determine the precise efficiency of labor in the different places which he has in mind, and can put the matter into dollars and cents for comparison with the cost of transportation, raw ma- terials, and other factors. Meanwhile, our map makes no claim to be more than a first approximation to the truth. Therefore no maps of individual continents are now presented, but merely a map of the world, Figure 22, and of the United States, Figure 34. In preparing these the figures for Hann's stations have been placed on the maps. Then a line has been drawn to include all places falling not more than 25 points below the possible maximum. These are ranked as "very high" and are shaded black. A second line includes places falling from 25 to 50 below the maximum, and the area thus delimited is ranked as "high," and shaded with heavy black lines. The next division, indicated by light lines, is ranked as "medium," and the values range from 50 to 75 below the maximum. The area shaded with thickly scattered spots includes places ranging from 75 to 115 below maximum, and is counted as low. The fifth division, shaded with widely scattered spots, is "very low," and ranges from 115 to 155. Finally, the hot desert areas which fall below 155 are left unshaded, but if humidity were considered they would probably rank as high as the wet parts of the tropics. Let us now make a similar map on the basis of health. For this purpose I have used the climograph for the eastern United States as given in World Power and Evolution. Each column and each horizontal line of the original climograph was plotted, smoothed, and prolonged to the necessary limits of temperature or humidity where these exceeded those actually occurring in the eastern United States. It was assumed that a temperature of 100 F. and a relative humidity of 100 per cent would speedily THE IDEAL CLIMATE 233 cause death, and that a mean temperature of 120 for day and night, even with the lowest relative humidity, would ultimately be fatal. From the smoothed data thus prepared the departures of the death rate from the normal under any combination of tem- perature and humidity can be determined. On the basis of the data previously given it was assumed that the effect of variability is approximately equal to that of temperature or humidity. Since the best degree of variability seems to be about twenty-one storm centers per year according to Kullmer's scale, this number was counted as the optimum. It was assumed that places having this degree of storminess or more have an advantage in health equal to one half of that due to temperature and humidity com- bined. This combined effect was measured by the difference be- tween the two extremes of Yuma, 7.8 per cent above normal, and San Diego, 6.3 per cent below normal. Although equal impor- tance is thus assigned to temperature, humidity, and storminess in the United States, the importance of storms for the world as a whole amounts to only about one fourth that of the other two combined. Unfortunately the data as to relative humidity are scanty and lack uniformity. Excellent and uniform figures, however, are available for the United States and India, and less satisfactory figures for Russia and Siberia. These indicate that if complete data were available a map of climatic energy on the basis of the effect of the weather on health in the United States would closely resemble the one based on factory work, as may be seen by comparing Figures 34 and 36. Hence in the absence of good data as to humidity we may for the present use the map of climatic energy based on factory work. (Figure 22.) The simi- larity of this map to the Indian, Siberian, and American portions of the map based on our data for health and the weather in the United States goes far toward proving its general reliability. The most noticeable feature of Figure 22 is two large black areas of "very high" energy in the United States and southern 234 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE Jf9 MO KO 100 %0 60 40 JtO AO 40 60 So 200 1W Figure 22. The Effect of Climate on Human Energy as Inferred from Work in Factories Canada on the one hand, and in western Europe on the other. Each is surrounded by a heavily shaded "high" area of large extent. The remaining high areas, four in number, are surpris- ingly limited. One lies chiefly in Japan. It is shown as extending into Korea, but the correctness of this is doubtful, for the records of storms in that region are imperfect. The second lies chiefly in New Zealand, but extends into Australia. The records of storms in this region have been published less fully than in Europe and America, but the general appearance of the map seems to be approximately correct. The third of the minor high areas is located in the southern part of South America. The records here are very imperfect, and the extent of the high area is doubtful. The reason for this uncertainty is not only that THE IDEAL CLIMATE 235 reliable records of storms are not abundant, but that the avail- able data do not enable us to determine how much change from day to day is caused by the average storm. The amount of change must be slight compared with that experienced under similar circumstances in North America and Eurasia, because no part of the southern end of South America is far from the ocean. The fourth of the minor high areas lies along the Pacific coast of North America. As already stated, its southern portion owes its character not to storms or seasonal changes, but to frequent breezes blowing in from the ocean. It extends only a short distance inland, and is too narrow to be prominent on the small-scale maps of this volume. In the far North human energy appears to decline more than would be expected. We know that population is scanty, and civilization low, but we commonly ascribe this to the difficulties of agriculture. Little can be demanded of people who must get a living by hunting and fishing. From the map, however, it ap- pears that even if other circumstances were favorable we should not look for any great achievements. This accords with the slow, inefficient character of the Eskimos, and of the Ostiaks and other inhabitants of northern Siberia. Grenfell in his book on Labora- dor says that the Eskimo "cannot compare with the Newfound- land white fisherman for perseverance and 'snap.' An Eskimo does not get one fish for the other's ten." This happens even when the Eskimo is in his native habitat, and is doing work to which he has been trained from childhood. Racial inheritance may have much to do with this, but the testimony of white men is that after a long stay in the Arctic they themselves lose ambi- tion and energy. At the other extreme of climate the regions within thirty de- grees of the equator seem to be characterized by essentially the conditions that we should expect. The status of the highlands is striking. A high degree of energy among white men would not be expected permanently in any of them. We are often told that 236 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE the climate of tropical highlands is as fine as any in the world. Not infrequently people are urged to colonize such regions. In book after book we read that there is not the slightest reason why the white man should not live there as well as at home. I do not assert that this may not be possible. In fact, I strongly hope that some day it will come to pass. Nevertheless, our map seems to indicate that previous to any such desirable consummation we must greatly increase our knowledge of how to adapt our- selves to nature, and especially of how to select physical types which are capable of preserving their own health and raising children in spite of the monotonous climate. At present, while the white man may learn to preserve his health in tropical regions, he can scarcely expect to retain the vigor which he dis- plays in the more favored parts of Europe. The most unexpected feature of the map is the diminution of energy as one proceeds eastward from western Europe to central Asia. In the deserts of Turkestan and Mongolia, and especially in the Tibetan highland, the map should probably show lower conditions than are actually depicted, but as records are not available, the medium shading has been extended across the whole of the unknown area. Before making the studies here described I should have said that a man in Siberia could be as efficient as in far western Russia in the same latitude. Yet the Baltic Prov- inces are very high in climatic energy, while eastward there is a steady decline until only medium conditions are reached. The reason is readily apparent. In the first place, the Siberian winter is colder and longer than that of the region near the Baltic Sea. More important than this, however, is the decline in storminess as one passes eastward across Russia into Siberia. The cyclonic centers of low pressure, which constitute storms, are either broken up when they approach Asia in winter, or else swing out toward the sea to avoid the great area of high barometric pres- sure which lies over the continent during the cold season. Hence, during midwinter the far interior is characterized by clear and THE IDEAL CLIMATE 287 extremely cold weather, not hard to bear, but steadily benumb- ing. In the spring and autumn, on the other hand, storms are fairly frequent, and are often of most terrific intensity. The burans, as they are called, are even worse than our western blizzards, which are the same thing under another name. They destroy cattle and horses by the thousand, and human beings often perish within a hundred yards of their houses. Only when the burans are at an end and the milder storms of the late spring and summer prevail does Siberia enjoy a highly stimulating climate. The conditions just described afford an interesting commen- tary on the common idea that the plains of Siberia are to be the scene of a wonderful development of European civilization during the next few centuries. I formerly shared this opinion, but have now been obliged to modify it. While this chapter was being written I spoke of this change of opinion to a Russian friend who has come to America for the sake of greater freedom. "Yes," he said, "that is just what the exiles say. I have many friends who are exiles. When they are sent to Siberia they take books with them and expect to do much work in writing and along other lines. Some plan to carry on linguistic studies, and some to make various other kinds of scientific investigations, but they almost never do it. They say that at first they begin to work with great vigor, but after a year or two their energy de- clines. They have the desire to work, but do not seem able to do so. They attribute this to being so far from home, and to the lack of stimulating contact with civilization. I think there may be more to it than that, for they seem to lose their energy." Nansen, in his fine book, Through Siberia: The Land of the Future, emphasizes this point. He frequently speaks of the slow- ness and inertia which he encountered. Here per acre the Sibe- rians raise far smaller crops than the Norwegians, and the main reason assigned by Nansen is lack of care, forethought, and energy in cultivating and fertilizing the soil. "There is no hurry 238 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE here," he says, "Siberia is still a country that has a super- abundance of time, as of everything else ; they may think them- selves lucky for having so far escaped the nervous stress that we know too well in Europe." He also quotes Rodishev, one of the most enlightened members of the Duma, who sums up his impressions of a journey in Siberia with the statement that the Siberians are "a people without enterprise or initiative." Yet the vast body of Russians in Siberia consists of pioneers who voluntarily went to the new country. The mere fact that they broke old ties and made the hard journey into the wilderness bespeaks more than the usual degree of energy and initiative. Similar people in the United States and Canada display great activity ; in Siberia the climate seems to damp their vigor. All this suggests that the old Russian autocracy accomplished its purpose more fully than it realized. It not only exiled many of its most thoughtful and active people, but sent them to a place where not only do the isolation and hardships diminish their power, but where nature insidiously accomplishes exactly the kind of repression that the authorities desire. From the standpoint of climate,, without respect to the many other factors which may cause quite other results, the relative positions of Russia and Siberia do not seem likely to change. Both, we may rightly hope, are destined to advance far beyond their present position, but while there is reason to think that western Russia may approach the standard of western Europe, Siberia suffers from a handicap which may never let her overtake the Baltic regions on the west of the great northern republic. Turning to China, we find that the summers are often debili- tatingly hot, with a steady, damp heat that is apt to be trying. The winters, on the other hand, are by no means so long as in Siberia, nor so severe. Yet they are far worse than in western Europe, and as bad as in any part of the United States. Cold waves often sweep down from the north, and are so severe that instead of being stimulants, they are depressing in regions like THE IDEAL CLIMATE 239 Peking. In the south, however, they are beneficial. Everywhere cyclonic storms are rare, so that there is no stimulus of great importance from that source. This is one of the chief reasons why China does not stand high on the energy map. The northern parts of the country are more favored than those in the south or in the far interior, but the difference is not great. Indeed, the uniformity of all parts is surprising. The disadvantages of high temperature in the south are balanced by those of low in the north. If China were part of a smaller continent her nearness to the moderating influence of the sea would help her much more than is now the case. All through the winter she is under the benumbing control of the vast continent to the west, which not only sends out severe cold waves, but prevents the passage of storms. Japan, on the contrary, does not suffer so much in this way. Extremes of temperature are 'milder than in China, and stimulating storms are frequent. Her greatest drawback is the long period of hot, damp weather in summer. Nevertheless she stands high. Here we must bring our review of the map of cli- matic energy to a close. We shall come back to it again when we have studied the distribution of civilization. CHAPTER XI THE DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION DOUBTLESS the reader has already noticed the striking resemblance between the distribution of climatic energy and of civilization. Look again at Figure 22 and see how the black areas agree with the places of highest culture. In view of this it seems advisable to construct a map of civilization to serve as a standard of reference. Only two methods appear feasible. One is by statistics ; the other on the basis of opinion. Both pre- sent grave difficulties. The statistical method will ultimately prove far the better, but it may not be practicable for centuries. For a fair estimate of the position of a country we need accurate statistics of education, morality, industry, inventions, scientific and artistic skill, wealth, pauperism, charity, crime, and many other aspects of human life which will readily suggest them- selves. No reliable figures for many of these things have ever been gathered in any part of the world ; no statistics for any of them have ever been gathered in many countries. Statistically it is almost impossible to compare Afghanistan, for example, with Kamchatka. Even where accurate statistics are available, the methods of compiling them are often so diverse as to make comparisons misleading. We may know exactly how many people are arrested and convicted for theft in half a dozen countries, but in one country the police may be so inefficient that few criminals are apprehended, while in another practically every thief may be caught. Thus the better may easily appear the worse. The only way to use statistics at present seems to be as a check upon the other method. We can select some country so DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION extensive that its various parts differ decidedly, but sufficiently homogeneous so that the figures for all portions are comparable. Since the United States meets these conditions better than any other country we shall examine its statistics in several cases, and shall use them as a test of a map prepared on the basis of opinion. For a map of the civilization of the entire world we must rely on the opinion of well-informed persons, but we shall find that this agrees closely with the indications of statistics. The value of a map based on personal opinion depends partly on our defi- nition of civilization and partly on our confidence in the judg- ment of the persons in question. Even the best and broadest ex- perience does not eliminate personal or racial bias. Therefore, the only safe course is to obtain the opinions of many people belonging to different races and ruled by different ideals. Ac- cordingly, in the autumn of 1913, I asked over two hundred people in twenty-seven countries to help in preparing a map. Most fortunately this was before the great war broke out. Good feeling prevailed everywhere, and among men of sound judg- ment there was perhaps as little racial prejudice as at any time during the course of history. This is especially important because similar conditions may not prevail again for years. The persons whose assistance was asked were selected for various reasons. The larger number were geographers whose first duty is to know all parts of the world. Ethnologists in con- siderable numbers were included for the same reason, but they responded less freely than the geographers. Historians, diplo- mats, colonial officials, travelers, missionaries, editors, educa- tors, and business men were all included. The only criterion was that each person should possess an extensive knowledge of the world through personal knowledge, or, in a few cases, through reading. Some were selected because of knowledge of special regions not well known to most people and only reached by ex- tensive travel. To all these many kinds of people, numbering 213 in all, I sent the following letter : 242 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE "May I ask your cooperation in the preparation of a map showing the distribution of the higher elements of civilization throughout the world? My purpose is to prepare a map which shall show the distribution of those characteristics which are generally recognized as of the highest value. I mean by this the power of initiative, the capacity for formulating new ideas and for carrying them into effect, the power of self-control, high standards of honesty and morality, the power to lead and to control other races, the capacity for disseminating ideas, and other similar qualities which will readily suggest themselves. These qualities find expression in high ideals, respect for law, inventiveness, ability to develop philosophical systems, stability and honesty of government, a highly developed system of educa- tion, the capacity to dominate the less civilized parts of the world, and the ability to carry out far-reaching enterprises covering long periods of time and great areas of the earth's surface. "In preparing such a map it is evident that statistics may afford much assistance, but they need to be supplemented. They touch only upon material things in most cases, and none are available for a large part of the world. Therefore, our best resource is the personal opinion of competent judges. Accord- ingly, I am asking a hundred geographers, anthropologists, and other persons of wide knowledge, whether they are willing to take the time to divide the regions indicated in the accompanying list into ten groups according to the criteria mentioned above. Group 10 will include regions of the very highest character, that is, those where the greatest number of valuable qualities are found in high degree. Group 1 will include those which are lowest in these respects. On the basis of this grouping I shall determine the average position of each region and shall prepare a map accordingly. "The purpose of such a map is threefold. In the first place, it will prove intrinsically interesting to a large number of DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION 243 people, and is likely to arouse considerable discussion. In the second place, in all geographical, historical, sociological, and economic discussions it seems to me that we need a clearer, stronger emphasis upon human character, that is, upon the mental and moral qualities which dominate the civilization of the various nations. If this is so, it is highly important, in the third place, that we should determine much more fully than has yet been the case how far various moral and mental qualities are influenced by physical environment, race, historical develop- ment, biological variations, and other causes. In order to deter- mine these things we need a map which shall serve at least ap- proximately as a standard of reference. In discussing the influence of such things as racial character, differences of reli- gion, social institutions, modern means of communication, the form of the land, the relation of land and sea, variations of climate and the like, we shall be able to gain much light by com- paring their distribution with that of human character as it now exists according to a consensus of expert opinion. "The matter can best be illustrated by outlining a specific purpose to which I mean at once to apply the proposed map. [Here follows a brief description of the map of human energy on the basis of factory work.] "I recognize that those to whom this letter is sent will say at once that they are not sufficiently familiar with all parts of the world, and that they have no means of distinguishing between different parts of China, for example, or between the different portions of equatorial Africa. This is certainly true, but it must be remembered that the classification is very rough. It is only desired that the one hundred and eighty-five names on the en- closed list be divided into ten groups, no group to contain less than fifteen names or more than twenty-one, and each preferably to contain eighteen or nineteen. It may not be easy to determine whether all of the divisions of France, for example, fall in the first group, but it is perfectly evident that none of them will 244 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE fall in the fifth or lower. The chief thing is to place them as nearly as possible in their proper group according to one's own personal opinion. A given region may properly fall in the fifth group, but the purpose of this classification will not be defeated if it is placed in either the fourth, fifth, or sixth, for when the opinions of one hundred persons are averaged, individual idio- syncrasies will disappear. In view of the varying degrees to which each individual is familiar with the different regions of the world, I should be glad if each contributor would underline the names of regions with which he is well acquainted either by travel or reading, and would place question marks after the names of regions as to which his knowledge is especially deficient. Names not underlined or questioned will be considered as inter- mediate. The three grades of familiarity thus indicated will be weighted in the ratios of 3, 2, 1. [This has not been done, partly because only about half of the contributors made the division into three grades, and partly because the final results are not appreciably changed by using the unweighted values.] The grade of the various regions should be indicated by underlining or questioning the names upon the small slips mentioned below, but may be done upon the accompanying list if that is more con- venient, but in that case please be sure to return the list. For convenience of classification I enclose slips containing the names of the different divisions. These may be spread out upon a table and arranged in ten columns and shifted from column to column until an approximately satisfactory arrangement is reached. When thus arranged those of each column should be placed in the corresponding envelope of the ten here enclosed, and all may be mailed in the large addressed envelope. Envelope ten is for the highest group, and one for the lowest. "In making the classification, one or two points need to be borne in mind. In the first place, the past should not be con- sidered: Greece, for example, should be placed in the group where its condition during the past one hundred years would DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION 245 place it without reference to its ancient greatness. In the second place, if two races inhabit a given region, both must be con- sidered, and the rank of the region must depend upon the aver- age of the two, giving each one a weight proportional to the number of people. For instance, in a state such as Georgia where nearly half the people are negroes, they must receive half the weight. Still another point is that the rank of a country can often be determined by considering the position which its people take when they migrate elsewhere. For instance, the position of Syrians as compared with Germans when they migrate to Eng- land or the United States is a fair criterion as to the relative merits of the two races. After the first generation, however, this should not be applied, for the younger generation owes much of what it is to the new country. A final point concerns countries which are poor in natural resources, or which are not located in the main centers of the world's activity, but which are neverthe- less of high character. For example, so far as importance in the affairs of the world is concerned, England vastly outranks Scotland. Nevertheless, our estimate of the greatness of Eng- land owes much to the large number of Scotchmen who have gone out to build up the British Empire. Therefore, in estimat- ing the relative merits of Scotland and England, the matter of size or even of commercial importance should receive relatively little consideration, whereas the character and ability of the people as rulers, merchants, scientists, writers, and men of all sorts should have a predominating weight. "In publishing the final results I should be glad if I might print the names of those who have contributed, but of course this must be as each individual may choose. The individual lists will not be published, and will be treated as confidential. I judge that other contributors will feel, as I do, that their classifica- tions are of necessity so imperfect that they do not care to distribute them to the world at large. Hence, while the list of con- 246 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE tributors will be published unless the contributors prefer other- wise, their individual opinions will be withheld. I hope, however, to publish a list showing the average rank of each country and the range of opinion between those who put it highest and those who put it lowest. Inasmuch as the plan here outlined depends upon the cooperation of many contributors, no single individual can in any respect be held responsible for features of the final map which do not meet his approval. *"In addition to the general list of divisions, I enclose a set of cards bearing the names of the states of the United States, and of the provinces of Canada. Would you be willing to arrange these in groups and place them in the proper envelopes, employ- ing the same method as for the larger divisions? Group 1 will be the states or provinces which are least progressive, or least influential, so far as the general character of their citizens is concerned, and Group 6 the highest. Each group should contain about ten names. The object of this you see is to make a map of the United States and southern Canada on the same basis as that of the world, but on a more minute scale. "The rough grouping here suggested ought not to take more than a few hours' time. Many days, to be sure, might be devoted to it, but the added accuracy thus gained would not be sufficient to make it worth while. If you can give the necessary time at your earliest convenience I shall be most grateful. If you cannot, would you be willing to return the list, the slips, and the en- velopes in order that I may ask someone else to do it in your place? Whether you contribute or not, I shall take pleasure in sending you copies of the final results. Trusting that I may hear from you soon, I am "Very truly yours, "ELLSWORTH HUNTINGTON." * This paragraph was included only in the letters sent to Americans and to one or two Europeans especially familiar with America. DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION 247 Replies were received from 137 persons, while others sent copies of their publications, so that an answer of some sort came from about three fourths of those addressed. The majority of the remaining fourth were foreigners to whom a six-page letter in English might appear formidable. About 90 per cent of the English and Americans sent replies, which is a very large pro- portion as such things go. I am convinced that the rest failed to answer chiefly because the classification required more time and was more difficult than I at first realized. The fact that classifications continued to be received for an entire year indi- cates that many meant to answer, but put my letter aside for a more convenient season which never arrived. A third of those who replied, fifty-four to be exact, actually made classifications, and all but two or three conformed so closely to the general plan that it has been possible to use them. The names of the con- tributors are given in the Appendix. I take this opportunity to express the warmest appreciation of their kindness in cooperat- ing so cordially. Not only their classifications, but their letters were of the highest value and in many cases contained sugges- tions which have been of great assistance in preparing this vol- ume. The same is true of many letters from persons who did not contribute, but who took pains to explain their reasons and to suggest ways in which my plan might be improved. Except where direct quotations are employed I have not attempted to acknowl- edge my indebtedness for various ideas which distinctly modify the tenor of these pages. This is partly because the same thought was often expressed by several persons, and partly because in many cases I cannot tell from which of several letters an idea was derived. Except in two instances I have also refrained from mentioning names, because where so many have contributed materials of great value, it might seem invidious to mention some and not others. Therefore, I can merely express my gratitude to all concerned. The net result of this attempt at scientific cooperation among men of many races and tongues leaves a 248 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE strong impression of the spirit of fellowship and friendly help- fulness among men of wide interests in all portions of the world. The countries represented in the final classification and the number of contributors are as follows: Australia 1, Canada 1, Norway 1, Sweden 1, Netherlands 1, Russia 1, Spain 1, Portu- gal 1, France 2, Italy 2, Japan 3, China 3, Germany 5, Great Britain 7, and the United States 24. The number of Chinese and Japanese is particularly gratifying. The ratio between the num- ber of contributors and the number to whom letters were sent is higher among them than among any other main group except the Americans, as may be seen in the Appendix. It is to be re- gretted that no one from India or South America cooperated, and only one, a Russian, from the European countries east of Germany. The difficulty of making the classification is considerable. Several contributors spoke of spending an entire day upon it, or of taking out the slips time after time to arrange them more satisfactorily. Some said that they spent two entire days upon it. All seemed impressed by the way in which a systematic classi- fication of this kind brings out the weak spots in a man's knowl- edge. For instance, here is the way in which one contributor expressed himself : "One appreciates what a big world this is and how little one knows about it when he attempts such a task as you have set. It is a most excellent means of taking the conceit out of one." Another puts it in this way : "I must confess that it is the most difficult and one of the most humiliating games I have ever tried to play ! I always knew I was a fraud as a President of a Geographical Society, but I never knew before how great was my deception! The greatest difficulty I found lay through my ignorance of the proportion of the different races inhabiting a district." DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION 249 An interesting feature of the letters was the diversity of opinion as to the advisability of any such classification of coun- tries. To take the adverse opinions first, one of my best friends, an American geographer, put the matter very strongly : "I am complying with your request for a sorting of the slips you sent me. It is a very bad plan, and not, I think, of value. Indeed I am not sure that I would have done it for anyone else than you." I am glad to say that later he expressed a much less severe opinion. Another geographer, a Teutonic European, speaks most cordially in part of his letter, but comes out bluntly in opposition to this particular plan : "I am wholly unable to take part in this work. I take your scheme as a failure ... I guess you are here, like some other Americans, under the influence of a too systematizing spirit. It seems to me impossible to classify mankind by this simple method." Still a third, an American anthropologist, is equally uncom- promising : "Speaking frankly I do not conceive that the method you suggest is possible of scientific results. One must choose between statistics which are definite and mere judgments which are gen- eral. To apply the geographic method to a compound of sta- tistics and loose generalization may be productive of grave error." And a fourth, also an American anthropologist, expresses himself as follows : "It has been my endeavor, in my anthropological studies, to follow the same principles that are laid down for natural sciences ; and the first condition of progress is therefore to elimi- nate the element of subjective value; not that I wish to deny 250 CIVILIZATION AND CLIMATE that there are values, but it seems to me necessary to eliminate the peculiar combination of the development of cultural forms and the intrusion of the idea of our estimate of their value, which has nothing to do with these forms. It seems to my mind that in doing so these obtain subjective values, which in them- selves may be the subject of interesting studies, but which do not give any answer to the question that you are trying to solve." Another anthropologist, this time a European, at the end of a particularly long and suggestive letter, expresses himself thus : "Taking all that I have written here into consideration, I think that if we were going to grind all the different regions of your long list in the same statistical mill, and to try to compute an average, a highly improbable and fantastic result would be obtained. For my own edification I put some of your criteria to the test, though in a different way. I drew up a list of twelve characteristics of the 'highest value, 5 in which I included sense for beauty in literature (belles lettres) and a few others, and then distributed them to eleven different regions of the globe. My familiarity with those regions by a long sojourn or travel and reading, covers a lifetime. To each characteristic for each region I assigned a number, from 1 to 1 0. I then added the differ- ent values or points to try to find a ratio, which might be called 'index of civilization.' I give it valeat quantum valere potest." The table possesses so many points of interest that it is in- serted below. At the end I have added a column showing the rank of the various regions according to all the contributors, as com- puted according to the system presently to be outlined. If the plan embodied in this table could be carried out on a large scale, it would undoubtedly be better than mine. The difficulty is that it requires a vast amount of work and a degree of familiarity with the various peoples of the earth which is found only among DISTRIBUTION OF CIVILIZATION 251 a few exceptional students who can almost be counted on the fingers. In course of time we may perhaps hope for a map based on some such minute study of human nature. Yet when it is before us, there is every reason to think that its general fea- tures, with which alone we are concerned in this book, will be almost identical with those of the map which we shall shortly consider. The reasons for this will be given later. One important point stands out in this table. I have given too little weight to the aesthetic side of human nature. In framing a definition of civilization I consciously thought of art in all its forms, but it seemed as if this were included in "the capacity for formulating new ideas and for carrying them into effect," just as science is meant to be included. Moreover, the course of history seems to show that every nation which rises high in other respects sooner or later experiences a period of high develop- ment in art, architecture, literature, and science. Nevertheless, these things should have received more specific recognition in my definition. To turn now to the other side of the question, those who be- lieve in the utility of the plan presented in my letter naturally do not feel the necessity of stating their reasons. Nevertheless, a considerable number take pains to express approval. For in- stance, a widely traveled Englishman thinks that "there are tremendous possibilities in all such attempts." An American, who is familiar with most of the countries of the world, says : "Permit me to say how heartily I thank you for engaging in this enterprise. Despite any and all sympathetic or hostile criti- cism of such a work, or the cheap and hasty or really valuable appraisal likely to be made, such a scheme will be invaluable to all students of human progress." Another American, who has spent a large part of his life in the Orient, is of the opinion that: "It is a fascinating and very significant set of standards by which you have asked us to group zss 8gggSS8S8SS jo xapin auiAvoqs (a} a 'sjaMog -u ut A^tiBaq f-H r- 1 joj asuas (ea^s^) aOOOOit-0^DG*0 ui A^Ti^aq i 1 4 joj asuag (papnir>ui << ^JB ui A^nBoq ^0005G*OC<5*-.Q*^i^ M joj asuag s A^ajBS |BUOS ^^OOSOJ^^^^O^.^ o -jod jo aajSaa I A^n^owi 33 2 JO pJBpUBJS ffi auaiSAii jo h sa[dK>uud jo t^^Ot-XODOOQCQ^'*^^. W H 00 < uoqtjonpa jo v. raa^sAs pado *Ct-OiODX^t-^-iG*r-t^ 3 w -pAap AtqSiH LH O [oj^uoo PUB OjCO^^-l^M-tr-H^r-, pB9\ o^ aaAvo j 5 * c 3 " sreaiB R w PUB spouad a